How Processing Modes Influence Consumers’ Cognitive Representations of Product Perceptions Formed From Similarity Judgements
EXTENDED ABSTRACT - The mental representation of objects, and the judgment of similarity among them, is an important area of inquiry in the marketing and consumer psychology literature. There is a rich stream of research that examines consumers similarity judgments to draw inferences regarding how objects may be represented in consumers minds (Glazer and Nakamoto 1991, Tversky 1977).
Citation:
Donna Hoffman, Piyush Kumar, and Thomas Novak (2003) ,"How Processing Modes Influence Consumers’ Cognitive Representations of Product Perceptions Formed From Similarity Judgements", in NA - Advances in Consumer Research Volume 30, eds. Punam Anand Keller and Dennis W. Rook, Valdosta, GA : Association for Consumer Research, Pages: 177-178.
The mental representation of objects, and the judgment of similarity among them, is an important area of inquiry in the marketing and consumer psychology literature. There is a rich stream of research that examines consumers similarity judgments to draw inferences regarding how objects may be represented in consumers minds (Glazer and Nakamoto 1991, Tversky 1977). Though consumers are known to adopt experiential as well as rational information processing modes (Hirschman and Holbrook 1982; Holbrook and Hirschman 1982), the importance of the mode in which consumers process information to generate these similarity judgments has been left generally unexamined. In the information processing literature, marketers assume consumer operate under "bounded rationality" (Bettman, Luce and Payne 1998) and limited processing capacity and implicitly assume that consumers process information and make decisions according to a rule-based system that interacts with features of the decision-making environment to produce decisions. However, considerable research in the cognitive psychology literature suggests that there are actually two distinct ways in which consumers process information: the associative (or "experiential") system, in which reasoning is based on principles of similarity and contiguity and the rule-based (or "rational") system, where reasoning is based upon the application of learned rules. These two systems may work independently but in conjunction, or may compete with each other to generate responses (see Sloman 1996 and Epstein 1994 for summaries). Since the rule-based system has some capacity to "overrule," although not completely repress, the associative system, it is not surprising that most research on consumer decision-making and information processing tends to assume some form of a rule-based system as the basis for the decision-making process. Nevertheless, research in psychology suggests that these two systems of reasoning differ according to the nature and extent of the information they use in processing and the responses they produce. In fact, even when consumers are explicitly primed to use a rule-based system, the associative system actually "intrudes" upon the response, even "neutralizing" it. This suggests that it makes sense to try and examine more fully the impact of these two modes of processing on consumer response. Of course, marketers recognize that different types of consumer information processing exist, but usually examine these differences in the context of outcomes (Shiv and Fedorikhin 1999). Importantly, some consumer behavior researchers (e.g., Holbrook and Hirschman 1982, Hirschman and Holbrook 1982, Epstein, Donovan and Denes-Raj 1999) argue that the particular information processing model a consumer adopts, experiential versus rational, could impact her mental representation of object or events. This suggests, as one example, that consumers cognitive structure formed from perceptions among individual stimuli, say from similarity judgments, might also different depending on the particular information processing mode adopted. Yet, studies of consumers cognitive representations of structure implicitly assume that consumers perceive similarity among products according to a rule-based, rational processing system. These studies have examined derived structures among products and found that the derived structures may vary by factors such as the type of product being judged, and how the judgments were made. Glazer and Nakamotos (1991) results on compensatory versus noncompensatory processing makes sense in the context of goal-directed information processing, but the question immediately arises: what happens if consumers are in an experiential mode? Thus, it remains unanswered whether the same pattern of results will be observed when an associative, experiential processing mode is in effect. Such answers have implications for research in information processing and market structure. Therefore, we examine in this paper how these two processing modes (rule-based/rational versus associative/experiential) affect consumers cognitive structures underlying similarity judgments. The central premise of this investigation is that the processing mode influences the information processing system that is invoked, which in turn influences the processing effort and the manner in which objects under evaluation are mentally represented relative to each other. Specifically, we hypothesize that under an experiential processing mode, the associative system of processing is invoked and the resulting cognitive representation of a set of objects will generate a similarity matrix relatively more consistent with a dimensional, spatial representation. On the other hand, when the same set of objects is evaluated under a rational processing mode, then some form of a rule-based system is invoked, and the cognitive representation will generate a similarity matrix relatively more consistent with a hierarchical, tree representation. More generally, we propose that the mental representation of objects is the result of an interaction between the true structure of objects and the processing mode adopted to evaluate them. REFERENCES Bargh, A. (1996), "Principles of Automaticity," in E.T. Higgins and A. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles (pp169-183), New York: Guilford. Bargh, John A. and Tanya L. Chartrand (1999), "Studying the Mind in the Middle: A Practical Guide to Priming and Automaticity Research," Handbook of Research Methods in Social Psychology, H. Reis and C. Judd (Eds). New York: Cambridge University Press. Bettman, J.R., M.F. Luce and J.W. Payne (1998), "Constructive Consumer Choice Processes," Journal of Consumer Research, 25, 187-217. Carpenter, Gregory S., Rashi Glazer and Kent Nakamoto (1994), "Meaningful Brands From Meaningless Differentiation: The Dependence on Irrelevant Attributes," Journal of Marketing Research, 31(August), 339-350. Epstein, Seymour (1994), "Integration of the Cognitive and the Psychodynamic Unconscious," American Psychologist, 49 (8), 709-724. Epstein, Seymour, Sean Donovan, and Veronika Denes-Raj (1999), "The Missing Link in the Paradox of the Linda Conjunction Problem: Beyond Knowing and Thinking of the Conjunction Rule, the Intrinsic Appeal of Heuristic Processing," Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25(2), 204-214. Fiske, S.T. and S.L. Neuberg (198, "A Continuum Model of Impression Formation: From Category-based to Individuating Processes as a Function of Information, Motivation, and Attention," Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 23,1-108. Galinsky, Adam D. and Gordon B. Moskowitz (2000), "Counterfactuals as Behavioral Primes: Priming the Simulation Heuristic and Consideration of Alternatives," Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 384-409. Galinsky, Adam D., Gordon B. Moskowitz, and Ian Skurnik (2000), "Counterfactuals as Self-Generated Primes: The Effect of Prior Counterfactual Activation on Person Perception Judgments," Social Cognition, 18(3), 252-280 Ghose, Sanjoy (1998), "Distance Representations of Consumer Perceptions: Evaluating Appropriateness by Using Diagnostics," Journal of Marketing Research, 35(May), 137-153. Glazer, Rashi and Kent Nakomoto (1991), "Cognitive Geometry: An Analysis of Structure Underlying Representations of Similarity," Marketing Science, 10(3), 205-228. Gollwitzer, P.M., H. Heckhausen, and B. Steller (1990), "Deliberative and Implemental Mind-Sets: Cognitive Tuning Toward Congruous Thoughts and Information," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1119-1127. Hinton, G.E. (1990), "Mapping Part-Whole Hierarchies Into Connectionist Networks," Artificial Intelligence, 46, 47-76. Hirschman, Elizabeth C. and Morris B. Holbrook (1982), "Hedonic Consumption: Emerging Concepts, Methods and Propositions," Journal of Marketing, 46 (Summer), 92-101. Holbrook, Morris B. and Elizabeth C. Hirschman (1982), "The Experiential Aspects of Consumption: Consumer Fantasies, Feelings and Fun," Journal of Consumer Research, 9, 132-140. Howard , John (1977), Consumer Behavior: Application of Theory, New York: Wiley. Johnson, Michael D. (1984), "Consumer Choice Strategies for Comparing Noncomparable Alternatives," Journal of Consumer Research, 11(December), 741-753. Johnson, Michael D. and Claes Fornell (1987), "The Nature and Methodological Implications of the Cognitive Representation of Products," Journal of Consumer Research, 14(September), 214-228. Karylowski, Jerzy J. and Krystyna Sharzynska (1992), "Asymmetric Self-Other Similarity Judgments Depend on Priming of Self-Knowledge," Social Cognition, 10(2), 235-254. Kuhnen, Ulrich and Bettina Hannoer (2000), "Assimilation and Contrast in Social Comparisons as a Consequence of Self-Construal Activation," European Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 799-811. Mandel, Naomi and Eric J. Johnson (2001), "Constructing Preferences Online: Can Web Pages Change What You Want?" Working Paper, Columbia University. McKoon, G. and R. Ratcliff (1995), "Conceptual Combinations and Relational Contexts in Free Association and in Priming in Lexical Decision and Naming," Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 2(4), 527-533. Novak, Thomas P. (1993), "Log-Linear Trees: Models of Market Structure in Brand Switching Data," Journal of Marketing Research, 30 (August), 267-287. Petty, R.E. and J.T. Cacioppo (1986), "The Elaboration Likelihood Model of Persuasion," in L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol 19, pp 123-205). New York: Academic Press. Payne, John W. (1976), "Task Complexity and Contingent Processing in Decision Making: An Information Search and Protocol Analysis," Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 16, 366-387. Pruzansky, Sandra, Amos Tversky and J. Douglas Carroll (1982), "Spatial Versus Tree Representations of Proximity Data," Psychometrika,47(1), 3-19. Ramanathan, Suresh and Geeta Menon (2001), "Dont Know Why, But I Had This Craving: Goal-Dependent Automaticity in Impulsive Decisions," Working Paper, New York University. Shocker, Allan D., David W. Stewart, and Anthony J. Zahorik (1990), "Mapping Competitive Relationships: Practices, Problems, and Promise," in The Interfaces of Marketing and Strategy, George Day, barton Weitz, and Robin Wensley, ed. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press, Inc. Shiv, Baba and Fedorikhin, A. (1999), "Heart and Mind in Conflict: Interplay of Affect and Cognition in Consumer Decision Making," Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 26 (December), 278-282. Sloman, Steven A. (1996), "The Empirical Case for Two Systems of Reasoning," Psychological Bulletin, 119(1), 3-22. Smith, Eliot R. and Jamie DeCoster (2000), "Dual-Process Models in Social and Cognitive Psychology: Conceptual Integration and Links to Underlying Memory Systems," Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4(2), 108-131. Sattath, S. and A. Tversky (1977) Additive similarity trees. Psychometrika, 42, 319-345. Tversky, Amos (1977), "Features of Similarity," Psychological Review, 84(4), 327-352. Wright, Peter L. (1975), "Consumer Choice Strategies: Simplifying vs. Optimizing," Journal of Marketing Research, 11, 60-67. Yi, Youjae (1990), "The Effects of Contextual Priming in Print Advertisements," Journal of Consumer Research, 17 (September), 215-222. Zhang, Shi and Sanjay Sood (2001), "Deep and Surface Cues: Brand Extension Evaluations by Children and Adults," Working Paper. ----------------------------------------
Authors
Donna Hoffman, Vanderbilt University
Piyush Kumar, Vanderbilt University
Thomas Novak, Vanderbilt University
Volume
NA - Advances in Consumer Research Volume 30 | 2003
Share Proceeding
Featured papers
See MoreFeatured
Deprivation of Control and the Phonetic Appeal of Brand Names
Jamel Khenfer, Zayed University
Caroline Cuny, Grenoble Ecole de Management
Featured
The Effect of Psychological Control on Temporal Discounting: Conceptual and Methodological Implications
Kelly Kiyeon Lee, Georgetown University, USA
Selin A. Malkoc, Ohio State University, USA
Derek Rucker, Northwestern University, USA
Featured
Dancing with Commercialism: Emphasizing Dramatism to Persuade
Yuxin Bai, Lancaster University, UK
Xin Zhao, Lancaster University, UK
Hayley Cocker, Lancaster University, UK