The Influence of Regulatory Focus on Consumer Information Processing
EXTENDED ABSTRACT - Regulatory-focus theory (Higgins, 1987) considers the different approaches people use to attain goals. This theory posits that there are two distinct regulatory systems for achieving desired end states, one concerned with acquiring nurturance and the other with acquiring security. Individuals who pursue what is termed a promotion regulatory focus concentrate on achieving their hopes, ideals, and nurturance needs by approaching matches to desired end states, whereas those who pursue a so-called prevention regulatory focus concentrate on satisfying their duties, obligations, and security needs by avoiding mismatches to desired end states.
Citation:
Rui Zhu and Joan Meyers-Levy (2003) ,"The Influence of Regulatory Focus on Consumer Information Processing", in NA - Advances in Consumer Research Volume 30, eds. Punam Anand Keller and Dennis W. Rook, Valdosta, GA : Association for Consumer Research, Pages: 116-117.
Regulatory-focus theory (Higgins, 1987) considers the different approaches people use to attain goals. This theory posits that there are two distinct regulatory systems for achieving desired end states, one concerned with acquiring nurturance and the other with acquiring security. Individuals who pursue what is termed a promotion regulatory focus concentrate on achieving their hopes, ideals, and nurturance needs by approaching matches to desired end states, whereas those who pursue a so-called prevention regulatory focus concentrate on satisfying their duties, obligations, and security needs by avoiding mismatches to desired end states. Empirical studies that have examined regulatory focus theory reveal a number of intriguing findings. Of focal interest is the observation that a promotion focus is associated with the use of more risky, creative strategies and both the generation and simultaneous endorsement of multiple hypotheses. By contrast, a prevention focus is associated with more conservative, cautious strategies, such as generating only a few hypotheses or selecting one hypothesis from a set (Liberman et al. 2001). The preceding findings suggest the possibility that different self-regulatory foci may be associated more generally with alternative information processing strategies. Specifically, promotion-oriented individuals may be more likely to process all types of information in a broader, inclusive fashion by noticing and attempting to integrate multiple or all elements of a message, including contextual data. Prevention-oriented individuals, however, may be inclined to process data in a narrower fashion by focusing on individual components of a message in isolation of others. This paper seeks to test the preceding hypotheses and thereby advance regulatory focus theory by shedding light on whether regulatory foci affect the adoption of such alternative information processing strategies. In particular, we apply these notions to an advertising context and hypothesize that if an ad encourages the adoption of a promotion focus, ad recipients are likely to produce more favorable product judgments when the ad displays many non-obviously related pictures that imply a fairly abstract theme as opposed to a simpler, unitary image that relates directly to the advertised product. This follows because the inclusion of such related ad pictures should provide promotion-oriented individuals the opportunity to adopt their preferred information processing strategy, namely, to process and attempt to incorporate in a broad, inclusive fashion many disparate pieces of information. Accordingly, this should induce more favorable product judgments. On the other hand, if an ad encourages the use of a prevention focus, ad recipients should respond more favorably to an advertised product when the ad features a simpler, unitary image that relates quite directly to the product as opposed to many ambiguously related visuals that imply a fairly abstract theme. This should occur because a simpler unitary image should enable prevention-oriented individuals to adopt their preferred information processing strategy, namely, to process information in a relatively narrow, unambiguous manner. Accordingly, such individuals should respond more favorably to the advertised product. The above-hypothesized two-way interaction of regulatory focus and the complexity of ad visuals seems to require a fair amount attention and elaboration of the ad in order for people to discern the relationship between those two factors. Thus, such two-way interactions are anticipated on rather specific, memory-based measures, such as judgments of specific product characteristics, which more forcefully encourage all people to engage in effortful retrieval of specific information included in an ad. However, other tasks, such as rendering overall affective judgments, can be performed in simple, relatively uneffortful ways that do not require consideration or retrieval of detailed information. For these tasks, individuals chronic inclination to engage in effortful cognition may determine whether specific information is retrieved from memory and considered. Therefore, we predict that three-way interactions of individuals Need for Cognition (NFC), regulatory focus and the complexity of ad visuals should emerge on ad recipients overall affective product judgments. Specifically, an effect of regulatory focus and the complexity of ad visuals should emerge on overall affective product judgments when individuals are high but not low in NFC. High NFC individuals who adopt a promotion focus are expected to report more favorable overall affective product judgments when the ad presents many related pictures that imply an abstract theme rather than a simpler, unitary image that relates directly to the target product. However, high NFC individuals who adopt a prevention focus are expected to report more favorable overall affective judgments when the ad displays a simple, unitary image that relates directly to the target product as opposed to many related pictures that imply an abstract theme. A study was designed to test these hypotheses as well as several other more specific ones. Four versions of a target ad were developed, with each version varying on two dimensions. One dimension was the complexity of the pictorial images contained in an ad. The ads featured either many related pictures that imply a fairly abstract theme or a simpler, unitary image that relates quite directly to the product. The second manipulated dimension was the regulatory focus encouraged by the ad. Specifically, the ad headline expressed and encouraged either a promotion focus or a prevention focus. Participants judgments of specific characteristics of the target product revealed the anticipated two-way interaction of regulatory focus and complexity of ad pictures. Specifically, when participants were encouraged to adopt a promotion focus, they reported more favorable judgments of the target products specific characteristics when the ad displayed many related pictures that implied a rather abstract theme as opposed to a simpler, unitary picture that pertained directly to the product. However, when participants were encouraged to adopt a prevention focus, they reported more favorable judgments of the target products specific characteristics when the ad contained a simpler, unitary image as opposed to many related pictures. Participants overall affective judgments of the target product, however, revealed a three-way interaction of NFC, regulatory focus, and complexity of ad pictures. Consistent with our theorizing, the interaction of regulatory focus and complexity of ad pictures was significant on the overall affective judgments produced by high but not low NFC individuals. This resulted in the same sort of two-way interaction on high NFC individuals affective judgments as that which was observed on all respondents specific product judgments. REFERENCES Cacioppo, John T., Richard E. Petty, and Chuan F. Kao (1984), "The Efficient Assessment of Need for Cognition," Journal of Personality Assessment, 48 (June), 306-7. Crowe, Ellen, and Higgins, E. Tory (1997), "Regulatory Focus and Strategic Inclinations: Promotion and Prevention in Decision-Making," Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 69 (February), 117-132. Fiske, Alan P., Shinobu Kitayama, Hazel R. Markus, and Richard E. Nisbett (1998), "The Cultural Matrix of Social Psychology," The Handbook of Social Psychology, (Vol.2), eds. D.T.Gilbert and S. Fiske, Mcgraw-Hill, Boston, 915-81. Friedman, Ronald S. and Jens Forster (2001), "The Effect of Promotion and Prevention Cues on Creativity," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(December), 1001-1013. Higgins, E. Tory and Orit Tykocinski (1992), "Self-Discrepancies and Biographical Memory: Personality and Cognition at the Level of Psychological Situation," Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18 (October), 527-535. Higgins, E. Tory, Christopher J. Roney, Ellen Crowe, and Charles Hymes (1994), "Ideal Versus Ought Predilections for Approach and Avoidance: Distinct Self-Regulatory System," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66 (February), 276-286. Higgins, E. Tory (1987), "Self-Discrepancy: A Theory Relating Self and Affect," Psychological Review, 94 (July), 319-340. Higgins, E. Tory (1997), "Beyond Pleasure and Pain," American Psychologist, Vol.52 (December), 1280-1300. Liberman, Nira, Lorraine Chen Idson, Christopher J. Carnacho, and E. Tory Higgins (1999), "Promotion and Prevention Choices Between Stability and Change," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77 (December), 1135-1145. Liberman, Nira, Daniel C. Molden, Lorraine Chen Idson, and E. Tory Higgins (2001), "Promotion and Prevention Focus on Alternative Hypotheses: Implications for Attributional Functions," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80 (January), 5-18. Markus, Hazel and Shinobu Kitayama (1991), "Culture and the Self: Implications for Cognition, Emotion and Motivation," Psychological Review, Vol. 98, (April), 224-253. Meyers-Levy, Joan and Alice M. Tybout (1997), "Context Effects at Encoding and Judgment in Consumption Settings: The Role of Cognitive Resources," Journal of Consumer Research, 24 (June), 1-14. Roney, Christopher J. R., E. Tory Higgins, and James Shah (1995), "Goals and Framing: How Outcome Focus Influences Motivation and Emotion," Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21 (November), 1151-1160. Trope, Yaacov and Akiva Liberman (1996), "Social hypothesis testing: Cognitive and motivational mechanisms," Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles, eds. E. Tory Higgins and Arie W. Kruglanski, NY, USA: Guilford Press, 239-270. ----------------------------------------
Authors
Rui Zhu, University of Minnesota
Joan Meyers-Levy, University of Minnesota
Volume
NA - Advances in Consumer Research Volume 30 | 2003
Share Proceeding
Featured papers
See MoreFeatured
Using a Meta-Analysis to Unravel Relative Importance of Postulated Explanations for the Endowment Effect
Peter Nguyen, Ivey Business School
Xin (Shane) Wang, Western University, Canada
David J. Curry, University of Cincinnati, USA
Featured
B8. Factors Influencing Collaborative Consumption Usage in the US market: An Exploratory Study
Pia Annette Albinsson, Appalachian State University
B. Yasanthi Perera, Brock University, Canada
Bidisha Burman, University of Mary Washington
Lubna Nafees, Appalachian State University
Featured
Understanding the Role of Gifts in Managing Marriage and Family Relations: The Case of the Male Phoenix in China
Jia Cong, Lancaster University, UK
Xin Zhao, Lancaster University, UK
Chihling Liu, Lancaster University, UK