Reducing Competitive Ad Interference By Varying Advertising Context: a Test of Network Models of Memory

ABSTRACT - Past research in marketing has demonstrated negative effects of competitive advertising on brand name recall, brand attribute recall and brand attitude (e.g. Burke and Srull 1988, Keller 1991). Such effects have been explained using competitive interference and the theory of spreading activation as theoretical grounds. However, the processes underlying these theoretical explanations have not as yet been subject to empirical testing. The present research aims at providing insight into the process of competitive interference by examining the facilitating effects of varying advertising contexts on brand name recall.



Citation:

H. Rao Unnava, Priyali Rajagopal, and Sekar Raju (2003) ,"Reducing Competitive Ad Interference By Varying Advertising Context: a Test of Network Models of Memory", in NA - Advances in Consumer Research Volume 30, eds. Punam Anand Keller and Dennis W. Rook, Valdosta, GA : Association for Consumer Research, Pages: 45-46.

Advances in Consumer Research Volume 30, 2003     Pages 45-46

REDUCING COMPETITIVE AD INTERFERENCE BY VARYING ADVERTISING CONTEXT: A TEST OF NETWORK MODELS OF MEMORY

H. Rao Unnava, The Ohio State University

Priyali Rajagopal, The Ohio State University

Sekar Raju, The State University of New York at Buffalo

ABSTRACT -

Past research in marketing has demonstrated negative effects of competitive advertising on brand name recall, brand attribute recall and brand attitude (e.g. Burke and Srull 1988, Keller 1991). Such effects have been explained using competitive interference and the theory of spreading activation as theoretical grounds. However, the processes underlying these theoretical explanations have not as yet been subject to empirical testing. The present research aims at providing insight into the process of competitive interference by examining the facilitating effects of varying advertising contexts on brand name recall.

According to the associative network model of memory, concepts in memory are represented as nodes with the relations between concepts as pathways linking the nodes. When a person is exposed to information about a brand in an experimental context, we assume that a node is set up that marks the experimental episode and that all information obtained in the experiment is connected to this node. The experiment node is connected to the brand name node through the product class and advertising context nodes. The attribute information of a brand in turn is uniquely connected to the brand name node.

An uncued brand name recall task may be viewed as activating the experimental node since the task requires the retrieval of all brand names that are connected to the experimental context. The activation that flows outward from the experimental node will be distributed across the pathways that are connected to it. Therefore, the more the number of pathways that emanate from a node, the smaller will be the amount of activation in each path. If no competing brands are present, then the product class cue and the advertising context cue together identify each brand uniquely and assist in its retrieval by diverting activation towards it. If interfering brands are present, the product class cue is not unique to any one brand name. Therefore the activation from the product class cue is divided between all the brand names connected to it. This causes a reduction in the amount of activation reaching each brand, and if the total activation is less than the threshold level, results in retrieval failure. This suggests that the greater the number of brands encountered, the lower will be the level of activation that reaches each individual brand.

Further the level of activation of a brand name is proportional to the number of pathways converging on it. The probability of retrieving a brand name is directly proportional to the amount of activation that converges on it; however, a minimum level of threshold activation is required for retrieval to take place. This suggests that brand name retrieval will be enhanced when multiple paths are laid in memory, which lead to the brand name. Varying the advertising context of the brand will result in greater activation flowing to that brand since more than one advertising context node will be activated for that brand as compared to competing brands. This leads us to suggest that varying the advertising context will result in an increase in the probability of brand name retrieval since the activation from these multiple nodes will converge on the brand name. In addition to the retrieval advantage, multiple contextual cues should also help in making the encoded information more resistant to competitive interference. This is because the presence of more than one contextual path will make the brand name more distinctive and therefore less susceptible to competitive interference.

We employed a 3 (one exposure vs. two same exposures vs. two varying exposures) X 2 (competitive interference present vs. absent) factorial design to test our hypotheses using one hundred and fifty undergraduate students as respondents. Our results partially replicate past research on the effects of varied context. We find that repetition enhances brand name recall in the absence of competitive advertising; however, when competitive advertising was present, repetition resulted in no more recall than a single exposure to the ad. The use of varying ad executions was found to result in greater recall than using a single exposure, but resulted in no greater recall compared to the same exposure repetition condition, when competitive advertising was absent. However, in the presence of competitive advertising, varying executions resulted in greater recall than both one exposure and repeated same exposures.

We were also able to provide further insights into the role of contextual cues as predicted by the spreading activation mode. First, we found that when subjects recalled the ad context, their brand name recall was significantly higher than when they did not recall the ad context. We also find that the likelihood of brand name recall given recall of the ad context is significantly higher in the varied context condition than in the same execution condition, which supports the prediction that the greater the number of contextual paths that lead to the brand name, the higher the probability of brand name recall. Finally, we find that the facilitating effect of product class cue on brand name recall is greater in the absence of competitive interference, which provides further evidence in support of the spreading activation model.

REFERENCES

Alba, Joseph W. and Amitav Chattopadhyay (1985), "The Effects of Context and Part-Category Cues on the Recall of Competing Brands," Journal of Marketing Research, 22 (August), 340-349.

Alba, Joseph W. and Amitav Chattopadhyay (1986), "Salience Effects in Brand Recall," Journal of Marketing Research, 23 (November), 363-369.

Anderson, John (1983), "Spreading Activation Theory of Memory," Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22 (June), 261-295.

Anderson, J. R. and G. H. Bower (1973), Human Associative Memory, New York: John Wiley.

Barnes, J. M. and B. J. Underwood (1959), "Fate of First-List Associations in Transfer Theory," Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 97-105.

Burke, Raymond R. and Thomas K. Srull (1988), "Competitive Interference and Consumer memory for Advertising," Journal of Consumer Research, 15 (June), 55-68.

Burnkrant, Robert E. and H. R. Unnava (1987), "Effect of Variation in Message Execution on the Learning of Repeated Brand Information," in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 14, M. Wallendorf and P. Anderson (Eds.) Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, 173-176.

Cacioppo, John T. and Richard E. Petty (1979), "The Effects of Message Repetition and Position on Cognitive Response, Recall and Persuasion," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(1), 97-109.

Chandler, C. C. (1989), "Specific Retroactive Interference in Modified Recognition Tests: Evidence for an Unknown Cause of Interference," Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 15 (1), 256-265.

Collins, Allan M. and Elizabeth F. Loftus (1975), "A Spreading Activation Theory of Semantic Processing," Psychological Review, 82(6), 407-428.

Keller, Kevin L. (1987), "Memory in Advertising: The Effect of Advertising Memory Cues on Brand Evaluations," Journal of Consumer Research, 14 (December), 316-333.

Keller, Kevin L. (1991), "Memory and Evaluation Effects in Competitive Advertising Environments," Journal of Consumer Research, 17 (March), 463-476.

Lynch, John G. Jr., Howard Marmorstein and Michael F. Weigold (1988), "Choices from Sets Including Remembered Brands: Use of Recalled Attributes and Prior Overall Evaluations," Journal of Consumer Research, 15 (September), 169-184.

Mandese, Joe (1991), "Rival Spots Cluttering TV," Advertising Age, November 18.

McGeoch, J. A. (1942), The Psychology of Human Learning, New York: Longman’s Green.

Melton, A. W. and J. M. Irwin (1940), "The Influence of Degree of Interpolated learning on Retroactive Inhibition and Overt Transfer of Specific Responses," American Journal of Psychology, 53, 157-173.

Mumane, Kevin and Richard M. Shiffrin (1991), "Interference and the Representation of Events in Memory," Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 17 (5), 855-874.

Nedungadi , Prakash (1990), "Recall and Consumer Consideration Sets: Influencing Choice without Altering Brand Evaluations," Journal of Consumer Research, 17 (December), 263-276.

Ray, Michael L. and Alan G. Sawyer (1971), "Repetition in Media Models: A Laboratory Technique," Journal of Marketing Research, 13 (February), 20-29.

Raiijmakers, J. G. W. and R. M. Shiffrin (1981), "Search of Associative Memory," Psychological Review, 88, 93-134.

Russell, J. Thomas and W. Ronald Lane (1993), Kleppner’s Advertising Procedure, Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.

Unnava, H. Rao and Robert E. Burnkrant (1991), "Effects of Repeating Varied Ad Executions on Brand Name Memory," Journal of Marketing Research, 28 (November), 406-416.

Wilson, C. . (1981), "A Procedure for the Analysis of Consumer Decision Making," Journal of Advertising Research, p. 31.

Wilner, B. J. (1971), Statistical Principles in Experimental Design, New York: McGraw Hill Book Company.

----------------------------------------

Authors

H. Rao Unnava, The Ohio State University
Priyali Rajagopal, The Ohio State University
Sekar Raju, The State University of New York at Buffalo



Volume

NA - Advances in Consumer Research Volume 30 | 2003



Share Proceeding

Featured papers

See More

Featured

Communicate Healthiness Through Indirect Measures: The Effect of Food in Motion Figure on the Perceived Healthiness of Food

Moty Amar, Ono Academic College (OAC)
Yaniv Gvili, Ono Academic College (OAC)
Aner Tal, Ono Academic College (OAC)

Read More

Featured

N8. Effect of Awe on Collectable Consumer Experience

Eujin Park, Washington State University, USA
Andrew Perkins, Washington State University, USA
Betsy Howlett, Washington State University, USA

Read More

Featured

Rejecting Moralized Products: Moral Identity as a Predictor of Reactance to “Vegetarian” and “Sustainable” Labels

Rishad Habib, University of British Columbia, Canada
Yann Cornil, University of British Columbia, Canada
Karl Aquino, University of British Columbia, Canada

Read More

Engage with Us

Becoming an Association for Consumer Research member is simple. Membership in ACR is relatively inexpensive, but brings significant benefits to its members.