Confidence in Thoughts to Commercials: a Self-Validation Analysis



Citation:

Pablo Brinol, Richard E. Petty, and Carlos Falces (2002) ,"Confidence in Thoughts to Commercials: a Self-Validation Analysis", in NA - Advances in Consumer Research Volume 29, eds. Susan M. Broniarczyk and Kent Nakamoto, Valdosta, GA : Association for Consumer Research, Pages: 471-472.

Advances in Consumer Research Volume 29, 2002     Pages 471-472

CONFIDENCE IN THOUGHTS TO COMMERCIALS: A SELF-VALIDATION ANALYSIS

Pablo Brinol, Universidad Aut=noma de Madrid

Richard E. Petty, Ohio State University

Carlos Falces, Universidad Aut=noma de Madrid

Thoughts generated by the receivers of advertisements often determine both immediate and long-term acceptance of persuasive communications (e.g., Cacioppo & Petty, 1980; Wright, 1981; Petty & Cacioppo, 1983). Most prior studies of cognitive responses to advertisements have focused on the valence and the extent of thoughts. Valence refers to the favorableness or unfavorableness of the thoughts, and extent of thinking refers to the number of thoughts generated. In general, considerable prior research has shown that the more favorable thoughts people have to a message, the more favorable attitudes are, and the more unfavorable thoughts people have to a message, the less favorable attitudes are (Greenwald, 1968; Petty & Cacioppo, 1986; Petty, Ostrom & Brock, 1981; Wright, 1973).

In addition to examining thoughts for valence and number, other categorization schemes have been proposed as useful (e.g., coding for the origin of the thought, target, self-relevance, and so forth; see Cacioppo & Petty, 1981; Shavitt & Brock, 1986). One feature of thoughts that recently has been suggested as useful is people’s confidence in their own thoughts. According to self-validation theory (Petty & Bri±ol, 2000), the relationship between thoughts and attitudes should be greater when people have confidence rather than doubt in their cognitive responses. In an initial series of studies on self-validation (see Petty, Bri±ol, & Tormala, 2002), we have shown that thought confidence is conceptually and empirically distinct from other components of beliefs such as the likelihood or desirability of the consequences the beliefs express (Fishbein & Ajzen, 1985). In the current research, using self-reported and manipulated thought confidence, we provide evidence that increasing confidence in thoughts can either increase or decrease favorability toward an advertisement depending on the valence of the thoughts elicited by the commercial.

In Experiment 1, fifty-six participants were exposed to an advertisement for a new car composed of relatively convincing arguments. Participants were asked to think carefully about the ad, to list their thoughts about it, and to rate the ad on a series of semantic differential attitude scales. Cognitive responses were coded by two judges as positive, negative or neutral toward the ad. Consistent with the strong arguments in the message, participants generated significantly more positive than negative thoughts.

The extent to which participants had confidence in the validity of their thoughts was measured after the thought-generation task with a series o 7-point semantic differential scales (e.g., certain/uncertain). Participants were classified as high or low in thought-confidence according to their self-reports. Participants who reported relatively greater confidence in their favorable thoughts held more favorable attitudes toward the ad (M=7.11) than those who reported lower confidence in their favorable thoughts (M=6.19), F (1,56)=3.71, p=.05.

In Experiment 2, we changed the product, the strength of the information in the commercial, and introduced a manipulation of thought confidence. Sixty-two participants were exposed to a relatively weak commercial introducing a new vacuum cleaner, and they were asked to think carefully about it, to list their thoughts toward it, and provide their attitudes toward the ad. Analysis of the thoughts revealed that participants generated significantly more negative than positive thoughts overall, consistent with a weak message. Nevertheless, because a small minority of participants generated mostly favorable thoughts, participants were divided into two groupsCthose who generated mostly unfavorable thoughts (71.7 %) and those who generated more positive than negative thoughts (16.7 %). This division formed a variable called Thought-direction.

The other independent variable was formed by manipulating the extent to which participants had confidence in the validity of their own thoughts. This was accomplished by asking participants to recall and write down prior situations in which they felt confidence or doubt. Prior research has shown that this manipulation following a thought listing can lead people to misattribute the high or low feelings of confidence induced by the writing task to the thoughts listed previously about the persuasive message (Petty, Bri±ol, & Tormala, 2002). Thus, this induction followed the thought listing task but preceded the measures of attitude toward the commercial.

Results of a 2 (Thought direction: mostly negative or mostly positive) X 2 (Confidence: high or low) ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of Thought-direction such that participants who generated mostly counterarguments held less favorable attitudes toward the ad than those who generated mostly favorable thoughts, F(1,56)=13.50, p=.001. Most interestingly, and as predicted by the self-validation hypothesis, a marginally significant interaction emerged between Thought-direction and confidence, F(1,56)=3.27, p=.07. The two-way interaction showed that participants who generated mostly counterarguments tended to report less favorable attitudes toward the ad with high (M=3.11) rather than low (M=4.09) confidence. But, for the positive-thoughts group, participants tended to report more favorable attitudes toward the ad under high (M=6.37) rather than low (M=5.78) confidence.

The present research shows that increasing people’s confidence in their thoughts in response to an advertisement can increase or decrease the impact of the commercial depending on the dominant valence of the thoughts elicited by the ad. When thoughts were mostly favorable, increasing confidence rendered attitudes more favorable (Studies 1 and 2), but when thoughts were mostly unfavorable, increasing confidence rendered attitudes less favorable (Study 2). Thus, the current research suggests that researchers might find it useful to assess not only the number and valence of thoughts to commercials, as is common in existing work on cognitive responses, but also to assess the confidence people have in their thoughts. Just as measures of attitude confidence and other attitude strength indicators have helped improve our understanding of the link between attitudes and behavior (see Fazio & Zanna, 1978; Petty & Krosnick, 1995), measures of thought confidence might help to improve our understanding of the link between thoughts and attitudes.

REFERENCES

Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1980). Persuasiveness of Communication is Affected by Exposure Frequency and Message Quality: A Theoretical and Empirical Analysis of Persisting Attitude Change. In J. H. Leigh & C. R. Martin (Eds.), Current Issues and Research in Advertising, (pp. 97-122). Ann Arbor: University of Michigan.

Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1981). Social Psychological Procedures for Cognitive Response Assessment: The Thought Listing Technique. In T. Merluzzi, C. Glass, and M. Genest (Eds.), Cognitive Assessment (pp. 309-342). New York: Guilford.

Fazio, R. H., & Zanna, M. P. (1978). Attitudinal Qualities Relating to the Strength of the Attitude-Behavior Relationship. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14, 398-408.

Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, Attitude, Intention, and Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

Greenwald, A. G. (1968). Cognitive Learning, Cognitive Response Persuasion, and Attitude Change. In A. G. Greenwald, T. C. Brock & T. M. Ostrom (Eds.), Psychological Foundations of Attitudes. New York: Academic Press.

Petty, R. E.& Bri±ol, P. (February, 2000). Implications of Self-Validation Theory for Resistance to Persuasion. Paper presented in the first annual meeting of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology. Nashville, Tennessee.

Petty, R. E., Bri±ol, P., & Tormala, Z. L. (2002). "Thought Confidence as a Determinant of Persuasion: The Self-Validation Hypothesis." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 722-741.

Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T.(1983). Central and Peripheral Routes to Persuasion: Application to Advertising. In L. Percy & A. Woodside (Eds.), Advertising and Consumer Psychology (pp. 3-23). Lexington, MA: D. C. Heath.

Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Communication and Persuasion: Central and Peripheral Routes to Attitude Change. New York: Springer/Verlag.

Petty, R. E., & Krosnick, J. A. (Eds.) (1995). Attitude Strength: Antecedents and Consequences. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Petty, R. E., Ostrom, T. M., & Brock, T. C. (1981). Historical Foundations of the Cognitive Response Approach to Attitudes and Persuasion. In R. Petty, T. Ostrom, and T. Brock (Eds.), Cognitive Responses in Persuasion (pp. 5-29). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Shavitt, S. & Brock, T. C. (1986). Delayed Recall of Copytest Responses: The Temporal Stability of Listed Thoughts. Journal of Advertising, 19, 4, 6-17.

Wright, P. L.(1973). The Cognitive Processes Mediating Acceptance of Advertising. Journal of Marketing Research, 10, 53-62.

Wright, P. L. (1981). Cognitive Responses to Mass Media Advocacy. In R. E. Petty, T. M. Ostrom, & T. C. Brock (Eds.), Cognitive Responses in Persuasion (pp. 263-282). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

----------------------------------------

Authors

Pablo Brinol, Universidad Aut=noma de Madrid
Richard E. Petty, Ohio State University
Carlos Falces, Universidad Aut=noma de Madrid



Volume

NA - Advances in Consumer Research Volume 29 | 2002



Share Proceeding

Featured papers

See More

Featured

When the Ends Do Not Justify Paying for the Means: Consumers Prefer Shifting Costs from Means to Goals

Franklin Shaddy, University of Chicago, USA
Ayelet Fishbach, University of Chicago, USA

Read More

Featured

A5. Trusting the Tweeting President: Inside the Donald's reality: Gaslighting, pschometrics and social media

Dianne Dean, University of Hull
Fiona Walkley, Hull University Business School
Robin Croft, Brunel University

Read More

Featured

I'm Scared, Want to Listen? Fear's Influence on Self-Disclosure

Anupama Mukund Bharadwaj, University of Washington, USA
Lea Dunn, University of Washington, USA
Joey Hoegg, University of British Columbia, Canada

Read More

Engage with Us

Becoming an Association for Consumer Research member is simple. Membership in ACR is relatively inexpensive, but brings significant benefits to its members.