Decision-Making Competence

EXTENDED ABSTRACT - In consumer choice, as well as other areas of daily life, the ability to make effective decisions is critical. These skills include extracting relevant information from the world, assessing one’s beliefs and values, and integrating these pieces with a coherent decision rule. Research has often found human decision making to be Asuboptimal@ in these areas. These limits are reflected in such anomalies as preference reversals, anchoring and (insufficient) adjustment, honoring sunk costs, poorly calibrated confidence judgments, and hindsight bias (Kahneman, Slovic, and Tversky 1982; Yates 1990). This research has been primarily experimental. Consequently, it has typically focused on understanding general cognitive processes, rather than explaining individual differences. Nonetheless, these tasks do show considerable performance variability, in the sense that some people do better than others. We ask whether these differences reflect consistent individual differences, perhaps to the extent of constituting a coherent construct of Adecision-making competence.@



Citation:

Andrew M. Parker and Baruch Fischhoff (2002) ,"Decision-Making Competence", in NA - Advances in Consumer Research Volume 29, eds. Susan M. Broniarczyk and Kent Nakamoto, Valdosta, GA : Association for Consumer Research, Pages: 434-437.

Advances in Consumer Research Volume 29, 2002     Pages 434-437

DECISION-MAKING COMPETENCE

Andrew M. Parker, Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University

Baruch Fischhoff, Carnegie Mellon University

EXTENDED ABSTRACT -

In consumer choice, as well as other areas of daily life, the ability to make effective decisions is critical. These skills include extracting relevant information from the world, assessing one’s beliefs and values, and integrating these pieces with a coherent decision rule. Research has often found human decision making to be "suboptimal" in these areas. These limits are reflected in such anomalies as preference reversals, anchoring and (insufficient) adjustment, honoring sunk costs, poorly calibrated confidence judgments, and hindsight bias (Kahneman, Slovic, and Tversky 1982; Yates 1990). This research has been primarily experimental. Consequently, it has typically focused on understanding general cognitive processes, rather than explaining individual differences. Nonetheless, these tasks do show considerable performance variability, in the sense that some people do better than others. We ask whether these differences reflect consistent individual differences, perhaps to the extent of constituting a coherent construct of "decision-making competence."

The domain of this construct is defined according to five key decision-making skills. Decision structuring involves the extraction of key components from an unstructured decision situation, including potential options, attributes, outcomes, and decision strategies. Belief assessment is the determination of the likelihood of potential outcomes. Value assessment is the determination of the personal value of those outcomes. Integration involves the combining of beliefs and values into coherent decisions. Finally, metacognition is the understanding of the extent of one’s own decision-making competencies. Assessment of decision structuring requires the use of open-ended methods, which were beyond the scope of the current study. Hence, we adapted seven conventional experimental tasks, tapping into the remaining four skills. These tasks also covered other common decision-theoretic distinctions, including accuracy versus consistency and judgment versus choice. They were administered to 110 respondents from the Center for Education and Drug Abuse Research (CEDAR), an ongoing longitudinal study focusing on the etiology of substance abuse. All respondents were male and 18-19 years old at he time of the current assessment. The CEDAR database includes assessment of these individuals every few years, starting at age 10-12, and contains extensive physical, social, psychological, and behavioral measures. The decision-making competence (or DMC) protocol was run at the end of the 18-19 year-old assessment, and took less than an hour to complete. Retest reliability data were also collected on a subsample of these individuals.

The initial hypothesis stated that the seven DMC measures would correlate positively with each other, forming a "positive manifold" across decision-making skills. The results showed all correlations to be either positive or very close to zero. An exploratory factor analysis of the seven measures extracted three factors that did not correspond cleanly with the hypothesized distinctions of skill set, accuracy/consistency, or judgment/choice. Hence, we focused on the first (and largest) factor, most strongly represented by the tasks of framing, calibration, and multiattribute choice (and perhaps reflecting global decision-making competency). This factor accounted for approximately 25% of the variance in the original data. In support of nomological validity, this factor correlated with measures of intelligence (e.g., vocabulary and executive cognitive functioning), cognitive style (e.g., self monitoring), risk behavior (e.g., drug use), and social/family influences (e.g., a positive peer environment). Notably, while DMC correlated strongly with the two measures of intelligence, DMC showed strong links with the other CEDAR measures, independent of intelligence. Perhaps the strongest such link was between DMC and risk behaviors, including delinquency, illegal drug use, and sexual activity at a young age.

From a theoretical perspective, the correlation between the seven DMC measures suggests a real underlying basis for non-normative behavior, rather than simply being the result of random error (which would suggest zero correlation). While the strong correlation with intelligence suggests some effect of cognitive limitations, the correlations with cognitive style may reflect the use of non-normative decision strategies (which may be responsive to training) (Stanovich and West 2000). Finally, the correlation between DMC and risk behavior, as well as social environment, provides nomological validity for these experimental tasks long assumed to reflect aspects of real-world decision making (an assumption that has rarely been tested).

From a practical perspective, if DMC involves skills (rather than traits), then training people to make good decisions might reduce risk behaviors. Identification of groups with particular decision-making difficulties could lead to tailored interventions, such as educational programs or decision aids (Baron and Brown 1991; Fischhoff 1992). The present research focuses on relative competence. However, absolute competence in making consistent, accurate decisions is central to such legal and medical issues as how competent are adolescents to stand trial and manage their own heath care. Translating DMC into a measure of absolute competence will involve establishing a benchmark for acceptable real-world performance, then relating DMC to it.

REFERENCES

Achenback, T. M., & Edelbrock, C. (1983). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist and Revised Child Behavior Profile. Burlington, VT: T. M. Achenback.

Arkes, H. R., & Blumer, C. (1985). The Psychology of Sunk Cost. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 35, 124-140.

Bar-Hillel, M. (1990). Back to Base Rates. In R. M. Hogarth (Ed.), Insights in decision making: A tribute to Hillel J. Einhorn (pp. 200-216). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Baron, J.(1985). Rationality and Intelligence. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Baron, J.(1988). Thinking and Deciding. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Baron, J., & Baron, R. V. (Eds.). (1991). Teaching Decision Making to Adolescents. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Baron, J., Granato, L., Spranca, M., & Teubal, E. (1993). Decision-making Biases in Children and Early Adolescents: Exploratory studies. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 39, 22-46.

Baron, J., & Hershey, J. C. (1988). Outcome Bias in Decision Evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 569-579.

Beyth-Marom, R., Austin, L., Fischhoff, B., Palmgren, C., & Quadrel, M. J. (1993). Perceived Consequences of Risky Behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 29, 549-563.

Birnbaum, M. H.(1981). Reply to McLaughlin: Proper Path Models for Theoretical Partialing. American Psychologist, 36, 1193-1195.

Birnbaum, M. H. (1985). Relationships Among Models of Salary Bias. American Psychologist, 40, 862-866.

Bromiley, P., & Curley, S. P. (1992). Individual Differences in Risk Taking. In J. F. Yates (Ed.), Risk-taking Behavior (pp. 87-132). New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.

Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The Need for Cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 116-131.

Cohen, S. & Hoberman, H. M. (1983). Positive Events and Social Supports as Buffers of Life Change Stress. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 13, 99-125.

Cole, N. S. (1997). The ETS Gender Study: How Females and Males Perform in Educational Settings. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.

Cole, P., Usher, B., & Cargo, A. (1993). Cognitive Risk and Its Association with Risk for Disruptive Behavior Disorder in Preschoolers. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 22, 154-164.

Dawes, R. M. (1988). Rational Choice in an Uncertain World. New York, NY: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

Donovan, J. E., & Jessor, R. (1985). Structure of Problem Behavior in Adolescence and Young Adulthood. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 890-904.

Dryfoos, J. G. (1990). Adolescents at Risk. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The Effect of Emotion on Cue Utilization and the Organization of Behavior. Psychological Review, 66, 183-201.

Eckel, C. C., & Grossman, P. J.(1996). The Relative Price of Fairness: Gender Differences in a Punishment Game. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 30, 143-158.

Eckel, C. C., & Grossman, P. J. (1998). Are Women Less Selfish than Men?: Evidence from Dictator Experiments. The Economic Journal, 108, 726-735.

Edwards, W., Lindman, H., & Savage, L. J. (1963). Bayesian Statistical Inference for Psychological Research. Psychological Review, 70, 193-242.

Endicott, J., & Spitzer, R.L. (1978). A Diagnostic Interview: The Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 35, 837-844.

Epstein, S., & Meier, P. (1989). Constructive Thinking: A Broad Coping Variable with Specific Components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 332-350.

Fenigstein, A., Scheier, M. F., & Buss, A. H. (1975). Public and Private Self-consciousness: Assessment and Theory. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43, 522-527.

Fischhoff, B.(1983). Predicting Frames. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 9, 113-116.

Fischhoff, B. (1992). Giving Advice: Decision Theory Perspectives on Sexual Assault. American Psychologist, 47, 577-588.

Fischhoff, B. (1993). Transaction Analysis: A Framework and an Application to Insurance Decisions. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 7, 53-69.

Fischhoff, B. (1996). The Real World: What Good Is It? Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 65, 232-248.

Fischhoff, B., & Beyth-Marom, R. (1983). Hypothesis Evaluation from a Bayesian Perspective. Psychological Review, 90, 239-260.

Fischhoff, B., Bostrom, A., & Quadrel, M. J. (in press). Risk Perception and Communication. In R. Detels, J. McEwen, R. Beaglehole, & H. Tanaka (Eds), Oxford Textbook of Public Health. London: Oxford University Press.

Fischhoff, B., Crowell, N. A., & Kipke, M. (Eds.). (1999). Adolescent Decision Making: Implications for Prevention Programs. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.

Fischhoff, B., Downs, J. S., & Bruine de Bruin, W. (1998). Adolescent Vulnerability: A Framework for Behavioral Interventions. Applied and Preventive Psychology, 7, 77-94.

Fischhoff, B., & MacGregor, D. (1983). Judged Lethality: How Much People Seem to Know Depends Upon How They are Asked. Risk Analysis, 3, 229-236.

Fischhoff, B., Parker, A. M., Bruine de Bruin, W., Downs, J. S., Palmgren, C., Dawes, R. M., & Manski, C. (2000). Teen Expectations for Significant Life Events. Public Opinion Quarterly, 64, 189-205.

Freund, L. S. (1990). Maternal Regulation of Children’s Problem-solving Behavior and Its Impact on Children’s Performance. Child Development, 61, 113-126.

Gardner, W., Scherer, D., & Tester, M. (1989). Asserting Scientific Authority: Cognitive Development and Adolescent Legal Rights. American Psychologist, 44, 895-902.

Giancola, P. R., Mezzich, A. C., & Tarter, R. E. (1998). Executive Cognitive Functioning, Temperament, and Antisocial Behavior in Conduct-disordered Adolescent Females. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 629-41.

Gilligan, C. (1982). In a Different Voice: Psychological Theory and Women’s Development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Graziano, W. G., Leone, C., Musser, L. M., & Lautenschlager, G. J. (1987). Self-monitoring in Children: A Differential Approach to Social Development. Developmental Psychology, 23, 571-576.

Hammond, J. R., Keeney, R. L., & Raiffa, H. (1998). Smart Decisions. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Business School.

Hedges, L. V., & Nowell, A. (1995). Sex Differences in Mental Test Scores, Variability, and Numbers of High-scoring Individuals. Science, 269, 41-45.

Hogarth, R. M., & Kunreuther, H. (1995). Decision Making Under Ignorance: Arguing with Yourself. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 10, 15-36.

Hollingshead, A. (1975). Four Factor Index of Social Status. Unpublished manuscript.

Jacobs, J. E., Greenwald, J. P., & Osgood, D. W. (1995). Developmental Differences in Baserate Estimates of Social Behaviors and Attitudes. Social Development, 4, 165-181.

Janis, I. L., & Mann, L. (1976). Coping with Decisional Conflict: An Analysis of How Stress Affects Decision-making Suggests Interventions to Improve the Process. American Scientist, 64, 657-666.

Jessor, R., & Jessor, S. (1977). Problem Behavior and Psychopathology. New York, NY: Academic Press.

Jones, S. K., Yurak, T. J., & Frisch, D. (1997). The Effect of Outcome Information on the Evaluation and Recall of Individuals’ Own Decisions. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 71, 95-120.

Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., & Tversky, A. (1982). Judgment Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Katz, L., & Epstein, S. (1991). Constructive Thinking and Coping with Laboratory-induced Stress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 789-800.

Keren, G. (1991). Calibration and Probability Judgments: Conceptual and Methodological Issues. Acta Psychologica, 24, 411-435.

Levin, I. P.(1999, November). Why Do You and I Make Different Decisions? Tracking Individual Differences in Decision Making. Presidential address to the Society for Judgment and Decision Making, Los Angeles, CA.

Levin, I. P., Huneke, M. E., & Jasper, J. D. (2000). Information Processing at Successive Stages of Decision Making: Need for Cognition and Inclusion-exclusion Effects. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 82, 171-193.

Levin, I. P., Schneider, S. L., & Gaeth, G. J. (1998). All Frames are Not Created Equal: A Typology and Critical Analysis of Framing Effects. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 76, 149-188.

Lichtenstein, S., & Fischhoff, B. (1977). Do Those You Know More Also Know More About How Much They Know? The Calibration of Probability Judgments. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 20, 159-183.

Lichtenstein, S., Slovic, P., Fischhoff, B., Layman, M., & Combs, B. (1978). Judged Frequency of Lethal Events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 551-578.

Linville, P. W., Fischer, G. W., & Fischhoff, B. (1993). AIDS Risk Perceptions and Decision Biases. In J. B. Pryor & G. D. Reeder (Eds.), The Social Psychology of HIV Infection. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Loeber, R. (1989). Values and Attitudes Towards Delinquent Behavior. Pittsburgh Youth Study. Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, PA.

Lopes, L. L. (1987). Between Hope and Fear: The Psychology of Risk. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 20, 255-295.

Luce, M. F. (1998). Choosing to Avoid: Coping with Negatively Emotion-laden Consumer Decisions. Journal of Consumer Research, 24, 409-433.

Mano, H. (1992). Judgments Under Distress: Assessing the Role of Unpleasantness and Arousal in Judgment Formation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 52, 216-245.

Mardia, K. V., Kent, J. T., and Bibby, J. M. (1979). Multivariate Analysis. London: Academic Press.

McLaughlin, S. D. (1982). Reply to Birnbaum. American Psychologist, 37, 1142-1144.

Murphy, R. O. (1997, November). Stability of Calibration Biases Over Time. Poster presented at the Annual Meeting of the Society for Judgment and Decision Making, Philadelphia, PA.

National Longitudinal Study of Youth. (1997). Bureau of Labor Statistics, U.S. Department of Labor.

Nunnally, J. C., & Bernstein, I. H. (1994). Psychometric Theory. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Overton, W. F. (1990). Competence and Procedures: Constraints on the Development of Logical Reasoning. In W. F. Overton (Ed.), Reasoning, Necessity, and Logic (pp. 1-32). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Parker, A. M., Downs, J. S., Fischhoff, B., Bruine de Bruin, W., & Dawes, R. M. (2000). The Appropriateness of Adolescents’ Confidence in Their Knowledge: AIDS-related and General. Manuscript submitted for publication.

Parrill-Burnstein, M. (1978). Teaching Kindergarten Children to Solve Problems: An Information-processing Approach. Child Development, 49, 700-706.

Parsons, O., Tarter, R., & Edelberg, R. (1972). Altered Motor Control in Chronic Alcoholics. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 80, 308-314.

Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., & Johnson, E. J. (1993). The Adaptive Decision Maker. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Pelham, W., Schneidr, W., Evans, S., & Gnagy, E. (1987). Sustained Attention and ADHD: Vigilance Performance and Methylphenidate Effects in the Laboratory and the Natural Environment. Paper presented at the Society for Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, Sarasota, FL.

Peterson, C. R., & Beach, L. R. (1967). Man as an Intuitive Statistician. Psychological Bulletin, 68, 29-46.

Peterson, C. R., Ulehla, Z. J., Miller, A. J., & Bourne, Jr., L. E. (1965). Internal Consistency of Subjective Probabilities. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70, 526-533.

Plous, S. (1993). The Psychology of Judgment and Decision Making. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Porteus, S. (1965). Porteus Maze Test: Fifty Year’s Application. Palo Alto: Pacific Books.

Poulton, E. C. (1982). Biases in Quantitative Judgments. Applied Ergonomics, 13, 31-42.

Quadrel, M. J., Fischhoff, B., & Davis, W. (1993). Adolescent (in)Vulnerability. American Psychologist, 48, 102-116.

Resnick, M. D., Bearman, P. S., Blum, R. W., Bauman, K. E., Harris, K. M., Jones, J., Tabor, J., Beuhring, T., Sieving, R. E., Shaw, M., Ireland, M., Bearinger, L. H., & Udry, J. R. (1997). Protecting Adolescents from Harm: Findings from the National Longitudinal Study on Adolescent Health. Journal of the American Medical Association, 278, 823-32.

Roelofsma, P. H. M. P., & Keren, G. (1995). Framing and Time-inconsistent Preferences. In J.-P. Caverni, M. Bar-Hillel, F. H. Barron, & H. Jungermann (Eds.), Contributions to Decision MakingBI. New York, NY: Elsevier Science B. V.

Shafir, E. (1993). Choosing Versus Rejecting: Why Some Options are Both Better and Worse than Others. Memory and Cognition, 21, 546-556.

Silverman, I., & Ragusa, D. (1992). A Short-term Longitudinal Study of the Early Development of Self-regulation. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 20, 415-435.

Skinner, H. A. (1982). Development and Validation of Lifetime Alcohol Consumption Assessment Procedure. Substudy No. 1284. Toronto: Addiction Research Foundation.

Slovic, P.(1962). Convergent Validation of Risk-taking Measures. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 65, 68-71.

Slovic, P. (Ed.). (2001). Perception of Risk. London: Earthscan.

Smith, S. M., & Levin, I. P. (1996). Need for Cognition and Choice Framing Effects. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 9, 283-290.

Snyder, M.(1974). Self-monitoring of Expressive Behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 526-537.

Snyder, M., & Cantor, N. (1980). Thinking About Ourselves and Others: Self-monitoring and Social Knowledge. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 222-234.

Spitzer, R.L., Williams, B.W., & Gibbon, M. (1987). Instruction Manual for the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R. Biometrics Research Department. New York, N.Y.: New York State Psychiatric Institute.

Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1998). Individual Differences in Rational Thought. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 161-188.

Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2000). Individual Differences in Reasoning: Implications for the Rationality Debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 645-726.

Tarter, R. E. (1991). Child Report on Peer Environment. Unpublished instrument, Center for Education and Drug Abuse Research, Pittsburgh, PA.

Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D.(1981). The Framing of Decisions and the Psychology of Choice. Science, 211, 453-458.

Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1988). Rational Choice and the Framing of Decisions. In D. E. Bell, H. Raiffa, & A. Tversky (Eds.),Decision Making: Descriptive, Normative, and Prescriptive Interactions. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Wallsten, T. S., & Budescu, D. V. (1983). Encoding Subjective Probabilities: A Psychological and Psychometric Review. Management Science, 29, 151-173.

Wechsler, D.(1972). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for ChildrenCRevised. New York: The Psychological Corporation.

Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence ScaleBRevised. New York: The Psychological Corporation.

Whitcomb, K. M., +nkal, D., Benson, P. G., & Curley, S. P. (1993). An Evaluation of the Reliability of Probability Judgments Across Response Modes and Over Time. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 6, 283-296.

Wolfe, R. N., & Grosch, J. W. (1990). Personality Correlates of Confidence in One’s Decisions. Journal of Personality, 58, 515-534.

Yates, J. F. (1990). Judgment and Decision Making. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice.

----------------------------------------

Authors

Andrew M. Parker, Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University
Baruch Fischhoff, Carnegie Mellon University



Volume

NA - Advances in Consumer Research Volume 29 | 2002



Share Proceeding

Featured papers

See More

Featured

The "Healthy=Lighter" Heuristic

Nico Heuvinck, IESEG School of Management
Yi Li, Macquarie University
Mario Pandelaere, Virginia Tech, USA

Read More

Featured

Proximity Bias: Interactive Effects of Event Valence and Event Nearness on Probability Estimates

Jennifer Seok Hwa Hong, New York University, USA
Chiara Longoni, Boston University, USA
Vicki G. Morwitz, New York University, USA

Read More

Featured

C4. The role of attachment to a human brand in improving eating habits

Amélie Guèvremont, École des Sciences de la Gestion, UQAM

Read More

Engage with Us

Becoming an Association for Consumer Research member is simple. Membership in ACR is relatively inexpensive, but brings significant benefits to its members.