The Impact of Explanatory Style on Event-Induced Stress and Buying-Related Manifestations of Coping Behavior

ABSTRACT - The impact of event-induced stress on coping outcomes is well documented in the social sciences. Research in the social sciences suggests that when confronted with an event, individuals make inferences about its causes and meanings based on past experiences with similar events and information obtained from the environment. When an event occurs, the individual determines whether or not the situation is controllable then, based on his or her belief of contingency or noncontingency between alternative responses and outcomes, makes inferences that influence emotional and behavioral outcomes. Within a consumer behavior context, these findings are important since it appears that consumer decision-making and buying behavior may be influenced by the stress associated with events that occur in peoples’ lives. However, few studies have explored the relationship between events, stress and impulsive and compulsive buying, and virtually none have examined how an individual’s style of explaining events might impact emotional states and subsequent purchasing behavior.



Citation:

Julie Z. Sneath and Pamela A. Kennett (2002) ,"The Impact of Explanatory Style on Event-Induced Stress and Buying-Related Manifestations of Coping Behavior", in NA - Advances in Consumer Research Volume 29, eds. Susan M. Broniarczyk and Kent Nakamoto, Valdosta, GA : Association for Consumer Research, Pages: 251-253.

Advances in Consumer Research Volume 29, 2002     Pages 251-253

THE IMPACT OF EXPLANATORY STYLE ON EVENT-INDUCED STRESS AND BUYING-RELATED MANIFESTATIONS OF COPING BEHAVIOR

Julie Z. Sneath, LaGrange College

Pamela A. Kennett, University of New Orleans

ABSTRACT -

The impact of event-induced stress on coping outcomes is well documented in the social sciences. Research in the social sciences suggests that when confronted with an event, individuals make inferences about its causes and meanings based on past experiences with similar events and information obtained from the environment. When an event occurs, the individual determines whether or not the situation is controllable then, based on his or her belief of contingency or noncontingency between alternative responses and outcomes, makes inferences that influence emotional and behavioral outcomes. Within a consumer behavior context, these findings are important since it appears that consumer decision-making and buying behavior may be influenced by the stress associated with events that occur in peoples’ lives. However, few studies have explored the relationship between events, stress and impulsive and compulsive buying, and virtually none have examined how an individual’s style of explaining events might impact emotional states and subsequent purchasing behavior.

According to the model of inferential reasoning (Maier & Seligman 1976; Abramson et al. 1978; Aneshensel 1987; Oatley & Jenkins 1992), individuals have a certain inferential (i.e., explanatory) style that they regularly use to explain events. The model suggests that when an incident occurs, individuals are likely to explain its causes or meanings using either an 'optimistic’ or 'pessimistic’ style. Individuals with an optimistic explanatory style have a tendency to make inferences about events that are external, unstable, and situation-specific (events are caused by external people and circumstance, situation is unlikely to continue and will not affect other aspects of one’s life), while those with a pessimistic style tend to make inferences that are internal, stable, and global (event caused by self, situation is likely to continue and will affect other aspects of one’s life).

Although events can serve as antecedents to anxious feelings, and certain events may be more likely than others to elicit stress, studies of the event-stress relationship indicate that occurrence of an event may not be the key determinant of behavior (see Rabkin & Streuning 1976; Dhrenwend et al. 1984; Zautra et a. 1986; Seligman et al. 1988). However, how the individual evaluates an event has been shown to influence subsequent level of stress (Rahe 1972, 1974; Morgan et al. 1986; Kim et al. 1997), and emotion-induced responses to events have been linked to consistent and discernable coping behaviors (Abramson et al. 1978; Kobasa 1979; Lazarus & Folkman 1984; Seligman 1988). Furthermore, since appraisal of an outcome as controllable is stress-reducing (Lazarus & Folkman 1984; Kim et. 1997), how an individual ultimately responds to the occurrence of an event may depend upon the level of stress associated with his or her perceived control of and past experience with similar situations. Consequently, while factors such as perceived control, stability and globality may influence behavioral outcomes; it appears their greatest impact may be seen through their ability to predict the level of stress accompanying an event.

Studies have shown that under stress, individuals often make purchases in an effort to cope with and reduce anxiety through previously experienced means. Increased levels of stress have also been linked to compulsive behavior, including compulsive buying (Faber et. al 1986, 1988, 1989, 1992; O’Guinn & Faber 1989). A type of unplanned purchasing behavior, impulsive buying has been conceptualized as acts that are spontaneous and unplanned (Weinberg & Gottwald 1982; Rook 1987), often performed in anticipation of pleasurable outcomes that have been previously associated with performance of a behavior. Given the previously shown relationship between event-induced stress and other unplanned behaviors, it is conceivable that impulsive buying behavior may emerge as a consequence of event-induced stress.

In the current study, it was proposed that an individual’s style of explaining events (explanatory style) may be used to predict level of stress which, in turn, influences likelihood of certain buying-related behaviors. It was hypothesized that individuals with an optimistic explanatory style (associated with external, unstable and task-specific causal inferences) would be likely to exhibit lower levels of event-induced stress than those with a pessimistic explanatory style (associated with internal, global and stable inferences), and that the outcomes of these increased levels stress would be manifested through impulsive and compulsive buying behaviors.

Independent variables were measured employing previously-tested measures of life events and stress, and the effects of explanatory style on the event-stress relationship were operationalized based on the Attributional Style Questionnaire for assessing explanatory style (Peterson & Semmel 1982; Seligman & Schulman 1986; Seligman et al. 1988). Measures for the dependent variables (impulsive and compulsive behaviors) were drawn from existing scales developed by Rook and Hoch (1985) and O’Guinn and Faber (1989). Findings suggest that individuals exhibiting an optimistic explanatory style exhibit lower levels of event-induced stress, while those with a pessimistic style have higher levels of stress. A significant main effect was found (F=12.22, p<.001) and the correlation analysis revealed that the relationship was significant and negative (r=-.158, p<.01). The results of regression analysis indicated that increased stress led to a greater probability to engaging in impulsive buying behavior (F=111.263, p<.001) and compulsive buying behavior (F=85.565, p<.001), with the former explaining more than 12 percent (12.7%) of the variance and the latter more than 10 percent (10.2%). Tests for mediation indicated that both the explanatory style-impulsive buying and explanatory style-compulsive buying relationships were mediated by level of stress.

The present research suggests that the application of the explanatory style concept need not be limited to studies in the social sciences. It appears that causal inferences may also be used to explain behavioral outcomes in a marketing-specific context, offering healthcare practitioners and consumer advocates a more complete understanding of the forces that motivate certain purchasing behaviors. Further research concerninghow the explanatory style concept may be applied to consumer behavior studies is likely to provide a more complete understanding of why and under what condition certain individuals are more likely to engage in impulsive and compulsive buying behaviors.

REFERENCES

Abramson, L.Y., Seligman, M.E.P. & J.D. Teasdale (1978). Learned helplessness in humans:

Critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 32-48.

Aldwin, C.M. (1994). Stress, Coping, and Development: An Integrative Perspective. New York: Guilford Press.

Anderson, P.F. (1983). Marketing scientific process and scientific method. Journal of Marketing, 47 (Fall), 18-31.

Andreasen, A.R. (1984). Life status changes and changes in consumer preferences and satisfaction. Journal of Consumer Research, 11 (December), 784-794.

Aneshensel, C.S. (1984). Social stress: theory and research. Annual Review of Psychology, 18, 15-38.

Brown, G.W. & Harris, T. (1984). Social Origins of Depression. New York: Free Press.

Buchanan, G.M. (1995). Explanatory style and coronary heart disease. In G.M. Buchanan & M.E.P. Seligman (Eds.), Explanatory style. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum (pp. 225-232).

Burns, M.O., & Seligman, M.E.P. (1989). Explanatory style across the life span: Evidence for stability over 52 years. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 471-477.

Cohen, L.H. & Lazarus, R.S. (1973). Active coping processes, coping dispositions, and recovery from surgery. Psychosomatic Medicine, 35, 375-388.

Coleman, J. (1973). Life stress and maladaptive behavior. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 27, 169-180.

Collins, D.L., Baum, A. & Singer, J.E. (1983). Coping with chronic stress at Three-Mile Island: psychological and biochemical evidence. Health Psychology, 2, 149-166.

Costa, P., McCrae, R., Zonderman, A., Barbano, H., Levowitz, B. & Larson, D. (1986). Cross-sectional studies of personality in a national sample: stability in neuroticism, extroversion, and openness. Psychology and Aging, 1, 144-149.

DeRubeis, R.J., Evans, M.D., Hollon, S.D., Garvey, M.J., Grove, W.M., & Tuason, V.B. (1990). How does cognitive therapy work? Cognitive change and symptom change in cognitive therapy and pharmacotherapy for depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 58, 862-869.

Deshpande, R. (1983). Paradigms lost: on theory and method in research and method in marketing, Journal of Marketing, 47 (Fall), 101-110.

Dohrenwend, B.P. & Dohrenwend, B.S. (1974). Social and cultural influences on psychopathology. Annual Review of Psychology, 25, 417-452.

Dohrenwend, B.P., Dohrenwend, B.P., Dodson, M., & Shrout, P.E. (1984). Symptoms, hassles, social supports, and life events: problem of confounded measures. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 222-230.

Dooley, D.& Catalano, R. (1980). Economic change as a cause of behavioral disorder. Pschological Bulletin, 87, 450-468.

Dooley, D. (1984). Why the economy predicts help-seeking: a test of competing explanations. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 25, 160-176.

Engel, J.F., Kollat, D.T. & Blackwell, R. (1968). Consumer Behavior. Hinsdale, IL: Dryden.

Faber, R.(1992). Money changes everything. American Behavioral Scientist, 35, 809-819.

Faber, R., O’Guinn, T.C. & Krych, R. (1986). Compulsive consumption. Advances in Consumer Research, 132-135.

Faber, R. & O’Guinn, T.C. (1988). Compulsive consumption and credit abuse. Journal of Consumer Policy, 11, 97-109.

Faber, R. (1992). A clinical screener for compulsive buying. Journal of Consumer Research, 19 (December), 459-469.

Faber, R., Christenson, G.A., De Zwaan, M. & Mitchell, J. (1995). Two forms of compulsive consumption: comorbidity of compulsive buying and binge eating. Journal of Consumer Research, 22 (December), 296-304.

Folkes, V.S. (1988). Recent attribution research in consumer behavior: a review and new directions. Journal of Consumer Research, 14 (March), 548-565.

Gibb, N. (1995). The EQ factor. Time (October 2), 60-68.

Gillham, J. Reivich, K.J., Jaycox, L. & Seligman, M.E.P. (1994). Prevention of depressive symptoms in school children: Two-year follow-up. Psychological Science, 6, 343-351.

Goldenson, R.M. (1984). Longman Dictionary of Psychology and Psychiatry. New York: Longman.

Hinkle, L.E., Jr. (1974). The effect of exposure to culture change, social change, and changes in interpersonal relationships on health. In B.S. Dohrenwend and B.P. Dohrenwend (Eds.), Stressful Life Events: Their Nature and Effects. New York: Wiley.

Hirschman, E.C. (1992). The consciousness of addiction: toward a general theory of compulsive consumption. Journal of Consumer Research, 19 (September), 155-179.

Holmes, T.H. & Rahe, R.H. (1967). The social readjustment rating scale. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 11, 213-218.

Jessor, R. (1981). The perceived environment in psychological theory and research. In D. Magnusson (Ed.), Toward a Psychology of Situations: An Interactional Perspective. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Kelley, H.H. (1973). The processes of social attribution. American Psychologist, 28, 107-128.

Kessler, R.C., Price, R.H., & Wortman, C.B. (1985). Social factors in psychopathology: stress, social support, and coping processes. Annual Review of Psychology, 36, 531-572.

Kim, L., Sandler, I., & Tein, J.Y. (1997). Locus of control as a mediator and moderator of the Effects of stress on psychological symptoms of children of divorce. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 25, 191-199.

Kobasa, S.C., Maddi, S.R., & Kahn, S. (1982). Hardiness and health: a prospective study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 244-252.

Lazarus, R.H. & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, Appraisal, and Coping. New York: Springer.

LeShan, L. (1966). An emotional life-history pattern associated with neoplastic disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 125, 780-793.

Long, B.C. & Haney, C.G. (1988). Long-term follow-up of stressed working women: a comparison of aerobic exercise and progressive relaxation. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 10, 461-470.

Maier, S.F. & Seligman, M.E.P. (1976). Learned helplessness: theory and evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 105, 3-46.

Menaghan, E.G.(1982). Measuring coping effectiveness: a panel analysis of marital problems and coping efforts. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 23, 220-234.

Menaghan, E.G. (1983). Individual coping efforts: moderators of the relationship between life stress and mental health outcomes. In H.B. Kaplan (Ed.), Psychosocial Stress: Trends in Theory and Research. New York: Academic (pp. 157-191).

Mendelson, J. & Mello, N. (1986). The Addictive Personality. New York: Chelsea House.

Moran, C.H., Jr., Owen, D.W. Jr., Miller, A., & Watts, M.L. (1986). Variations in stress responses as a function of cognitive and personality variables. Psychological Reports, 59, 575-583.

Nicholls, J.G. (1984). Achievement motivation: conceptions of ability, subjective experience, task choice, and performance. Psychological Review, 91, 328-346.

Oatley, K. & Jenkins, J.M. (1992). Human emotions: function and dysfunction. Annual Review of Psychology, 43, 55-85.

O’Guinn, T.C. & Faber, R.J. (1989). Compulsive buying: a phenomenological exploration. Journal of Consumer Research, 16 (September), 147-157.

Peterson, C. & Semmel, A. (1982). The attributional style questionnaire. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 6 (3), 287-300.

Peterson, C., Seligman, M.E.P., & Vaillant, G.E. (1998). Pessimistic explanatory style as a risk factor for physical illness: A thirty-five-year longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 23-27.

Rabkin, J.G. & Streuning, E.L. (1976). Life events, stress, and illness. Science, 194, 1013-1020.

Rahe, R.(1972). Subjects’ recent life changes and their near future illness reports. Annals of Clinical Research, 4, 250-265.

Rahe, R. (1974). The pathway between subjects’ recent life changes and their near future illness reports: representative results and methodological issues. In B.S. Dohrenwend and B.P. Dohrenwend (Eds.), Stressful Life Events: Their Nature and Effects. New York: Wiley (pp. 73-86).

Rook, D.W.(1987). The buying impulse. Journal of Consumer Research, 14 (9), 189-199.

Rook, D.W. & Hoch, S.J. (1985). Consuming impulses. Advances in Consumer Research, 12, 23-27.

Rook, D.W. & Fisher, R.J. (1995). Normative influences on impulsive buying behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 22, 305-313.

Rosenbaum, M. & Palmon, N. (1984). Helplessness and resourcefulness in coping with epilepsy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52, 244-252.

Seeman, M., Seeman, A.Z., & Budros, Z. (1988). Powerlessness, work, and community: a longitudinal study of alienation and alcohol use. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 29, 185-198.

Seligman M.E.P. & Schulman, P. (1986). Explanatory style as a predictor of productivity and quitting among life insurance agents. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50 (4), 832-838.

Seligman, M.E.P., Kamen, L.P., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1988). Explanatory style across the life span. In E.M. Hetherington, R.M. Lerner, and M. Perlmutter (Eds.), Child Development In Life-Span Perspective. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum (pp. 91-114).

Seligman, M.E.P., Schulman, P., DeRubeis, R.J., & Hollon,S.D. (1999). The prevention of depression and anxiety. Prevention and Depression, 2, Article 8. Available on the

World Wide Web: http://journals.apa.org/prevention/volume2/pre0020008a.html.

Stone, A.A., Heider, L., & Schneider, M.S. (1988). Coping with stressful events: coping dimensions and issues. In L.H. Cohen (Ed.), Life Events and Psychological Functioning. Newbury Park, CA: Sage (pp. 182-210).

Tausig, M. (1982). Measuring Life Events. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 23, 52-64.

Teas, R.K. & McElroy, J.C. (1986). Causal attributions and expectancy estimates: a framework for understanding the dynamics of salesforce motivation. Journal of Marketing, 50, 75-86.

Tice, D.M. (1992). Self-concept change and self-presentation: The looking glass self is also a magnifying glass. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 435-451.

Thoits, P.A. (1983). Dimensions of life events that influence psychological distress: an evaluation and synthesis of the literature. In H.B. Kaplan (Ed.), Psychosocial Trends in Theory and Research. New York: Academic (pp. 33-103).

Ullman, L.P. & Krasner, L. (1969). A Psychological Approach to Abnormal Behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Weinberg, P. & ottwald, W. (1982). Impulsive consumer buying as a result of emotions. Journal of Business Research, 10, 43-57.

Weiner, B., Russell, D., & Lerman, D. (1979). The cognition-emotion process in achievement-related contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 1211-1220.

Zautra, A.J., Guarnaccia, C.A., Reich, J.W., & Dohrenwend, B.P. (1986). The contribution of small events to stress and distress. In L.H. Cohen (Ed.), Life Events and Psychological Functioning. Newbury Park, CA: Sage (pp. 123-148).

----------------------------------------

Authors

Julie Z. Sneath, LaGrange College
Pamela A. Kennett, University of New Orleans



Volume

NA - Advances in Consumer Research Volume 29 | 2002



Share Proceeding

Featured papers

See More

Featured

A10. Opting Opt-in or Out? Effects of Defaults on Perceived Control and Valuation of Personal Data

Iris van Ooijen, University of Twente

Read More

Featured

When Zero Prices Backfire: A Dual-Process Model for Understanding Consumers’ Reactions to Zero Prices

Fengyan Cai, Shanghai Jiao Tong University
Xiaomeng Fan, Northwestern University, USA
Galen V. Bodenhausen, Northwestern University, USA

Read More

Featured

Divorcing the Market

Deniz Atik, University of Texas Rio Grande Valley, USA
A. Fuat Fırat, University of Texas Rio Grande Valley, USA
Ebru Uzunoğlu, Izmir University of Economics

Read More

Engage with Us

Becoming an Association for Consumer Research member is simple. Membership in ACR is relatively inexpensive, but brings significant benefits to its members.