The Impact of Background Lyrics on Recall of Concurrently Presented Verbal Information in an Advertising Context
Citation:
G. Douglas Olsen and Richard D. Johnson (2002) ,"The Impact of Background Lyrics on Recall of Concurrently Presented Verbal Information in an Advertising Context", in NA - Advances in Consumer Research Volume 29, eds. Susan M. Broniarczyk and Kent Nakamoto, Valdosta, GA : Association for Consumer Research, Pages: 147-148.
Music in advertising has generated considerable research in marketing (e.g., Alpert and Alpert 1990; Bruner 1990; Dunbar 1990; Gorn 1982; Gorn et al. 1991; Kellaris and Cox 1989; Kellaris, Cox and Cox 1993; Kellaris and Mantel 1996; Kellaris and Rice 1993; MacInnis and Park 1991; Park and Young 1986; Stewart, Farmer and Stannard 1990; Sullivan 1990). It may, therefore, be rather surprising that issues pertaining to the impact of lyrics in advertisements have received little consideration. Yalch (1991) suggests that consumers may remember a slogan when it is sung in a jingle better than when it is merely spoken. Until now, no marketing research has examined the ability of lyrics in the background of the advertisement to capture consumers attention, or the potential for information contained in background lyrics to interfere with the processing of concurrently presented verbal information. Information Processing theory suggests that lyrics in the background may be expected to interfere with attempts to memorize target material (see Belsham and Harmon 1977; Davies and Shackleton 1973); for example, recent research has demonstrated that irrelevant speech in the background impairs recall for concurrently presented information (e.g., Buchner, Irmen & Erdfelder 1996; LeCompte et al, 1997; Martin-Loeches et al. 1997; SalamT and Baddeley 1989). In a study more closely related to the research presented here, SalamT and Baddeley (1989) found that memory for target material is impaired by background music that contains lyrics, even when instructions are given not to attend to the song and the lyrics are in a language foreign to the listener. We propose that this finding cannot necessarily be applied to an advertising context, for two reasons. First, like most work in psychology, SalamT and Baddeley (1989) employed a directed learning paradigm; whereas, incidental learning (i.e., where participants are not aware that they will be required to recall information) is more typical in a naturalistic advertising situation. Second, the distracter music or speech is usually extrinsic to the target material in the typical psychology experiment (i.e., it is completely different from the information to be remembered); but lyrics are usually intrinsic to product information in an advertisement. Hence, the attention grabbing value of lyrics which interfered with processing of target information in the SalamT and Baddeley study (1989) might actually enhance attention to the advertisement and, therefore, increase retention for the target information. In Experiment 1, the presence of meaningfl background lyrics had a significant positive impact on recall for product information, compared to advertisements for which the backgrounds were just a rhythm section, an instrumental melody line, a vocal melody line, or nonsense lyrics. Experiment 2 replicated the finding that meaningful background lyrics enhance recall of product information, relative to the condition where only instrumental music is present in the background. However, memory was not enhanced when lyrics appeared at the beginning of the advertisement and ceased when the announcers script began. We interpret the results of the first two experiments to indicate that, in an incidental learning situation, meaningful background lyrics enhance attention to the advertisement and thereby increase memory for the target information. Experiment 3 examined recall in a directed learning task and found no differences in recall between the lyrics condition and the instrumental music condition. This is consistent with the interpretation that the benefit of background lyrics is to elevate attention to the advertisement. This heightened attention has no effect in a directed learning paradigm because the experimental task maximizes attention irrespective of background music. In conclusion, these experiments establish that background lyrics enhance memory for target information in an incidental learning task and suggest that the effect occurs because lyrics draw attention to the advertisement. These results are especially valuable to marketing researchers because they employ an incidental learning paradigm that is more similar to a natural radio listening situation, compared to the directed learning paradigm used in previous related studies. It should also be noted that background lyrics were never observed to harm recall, relative to conditions where no lyrics were present. Several interesting avenues for future research are suggested. REFERENCES Alpert, Judy I. and Mark I. Alpert (1990) "Music Influences on Mood and Purchase Intentions," Psychology and Marketing, 7 (2-Summer), 109-133. Belsham, Richard Lee and David W. Harman (1977) "Effect of Vocal Vs. Non-Vocal Music on Visual Recall," Perceptual and Motor Skills, 44857-858. Bruner II, Gordon C. (1990) "Music, Mood, and Marketing," Journal of Marketing, (October), 94-104. Buchner, Axel; Irmen, Lisa; Erdfelder, Edgar. (1996) "On the Irrelevance of Semantic Information for the Irrelevant Speech Effect," Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychology. 49A (3), 765-779. Davies, D. R., Lesley Lang, and V. J. Shackleton (1973) "The Effects of Music and Task Difficulty on Performance at a Visual Vigilance Task," British Journal of Psychology, 64383-389. Dunbar, David S. (1990) "Music, and Advertising," International Journal of Advertising, 9197-203. Gorn, Gerald J.(1982) "The Effects of Music in Advertising on Choice Behavior: A Classical Conditioning Approach," Journal of Marketing, 46 (Winter), 94-101. Gorn, Gerald J., Marvin E. Goldberg, Amitava Chattopadhyay, and David Litvack (1991) "Music and Information in Commercials: Their Effects With An Elderly Sample," Journal of Advertising Research, (October/November), 23-32. Kellaris, James J. and Anthony D. Cox (1989) "The Effects of Background Music in dvertising: A Reassessment," Journal of Consumer Research, 16 (June), 113-118. Kellaris, James J., Anthony D. Cox, and Dena Cox (1993) "The Effect of Background Music on Ad Processing: A Contingency Explanation," Journal of Marketing, 57 (October), 114-125. Kellaris, James J. and Susan Powell Mantel (1996) "Shaping Time Perceptions with Background Music: The Effect of Congruity and Arousal on Estimates of Ad Durations," Psychology and Marketing, 13 (August 5), 501-515. Kellaris, James J. and Ronald C. Rice (1993) "The Influence of Tempo, Loudness, and Gender of Listener on Responses to Music," Psychology and Marketing, 10 (January 1), 15-29. LeCompte, Denny C; Neely, Craig B; Wilson, Jeffrey R. (1997) "Irrelevant Speech and Irrelevant Tones: The Relative Importance of Speech to the Irrelevant Speech Effect," Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition. Vol 23(2), 472-483 MacInnis, Deborah J. and C. Whan Park (1991) "The Differential Role of Characteristics of Music on High- and Low-Involvement Consumers Processing of Ads," Journal of Consumer Research, 18 (September), 161-173. Martin-Loeches, Manuel; Schweinberger, Stefan R; Sommer, Werner. (1997) "The Phonological Loop Model of Working Memory: An ERP Study of Irrelevant Speech and Phonological Similarity Effects," Memory & Cognition. 25 (4), 1997, 471-483. Park, C. Whan and S. Mark Young (1986) "Consumer Response to Television Commercials: The Impact of Involvement and Background Music on Brand Attitude Formation," Journal of Marketing Research, 23 (February), 11-24. SalamT, Pierre and Alan Baddeley (1987) "Noise, Unattended Speech and Short-Term Memory," Ergonomics, 30 (8), 1185-1194. SalamT, Pierre and Alan Baddeley (1989) "Effects of Background Music on Phonological Short-Term Memory," The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41A (1), 107-122. Stewart, David W., Kenneth M Farmer, and Charles I. Stannard (1990) "Music as a Recognition Cue in Advertising Tracking Studies," Journal of Advertising Research, (August/September), 39-48. Sullivan, Gary L. (1990) "Music Format Effects in Radio Advertising," Psychology and Marketing, 7 (Summer 2), 97-108. Yalch, Richard F. (1991) "Memory in Jingle Jungle: Music as a Mnemonic Device in Communicating Advertising Slogans," Journal of Applied Psychology, 76 (2), 268-275. ----------------------------------------
Authors
G. Douglas Olsen, University of Alberta
Richard D. Johnson, University of Alberta
Volume
NA - Advances in Consumer Research Volume 29 | 2002
Share Proceeding
Featured papers
See MoreFeatured
Q5. Conceptualizing the Digital Experience in Luxury
Wided Batat, American University Beirut
Featured
Making the Wait Worthwhile: Mental Accounting and the Effect of Waiting in Line on Consumption
Chris Hydock, Georgetown University, USA
Sezer Ulku, Georgetown University, USA
Shiliang Cui, Georgetown University, USA
Featured
R9. The Asymmetric Effects Of Attitude Toward The Brand (Symbolic Vs. Functional) Upon Recommendation System (Artificial Intelligence Vs. Human)
Kiwan Park, Seoul National University, USA
Yaeri Kim, Seoul National University, USA
Seojin Stacey Lee, Seoul National University, USA