Metacognitive Beliefs About Groups
EXTENDED ABSTRACT - We look to others to tell us how to think, feel, and act. Researchers have a detailed understanding of how group norms affect group members cognition, emotion, and behavior. Surprisingly, however, much less is known about how people actually construct their estimates of collective sentiment (for notable exceptions, see Hamil, Nisbett, & Wilson, 1989; McFarland & Miller, 1990; Miller & McFarland, 1987; Prentice & Miller, 1993). At the same time, however, we know that perceivers often misjudge public opinion (Prentice & Miller, 1993). In this paper, we examine one reason why by exploring the process by which perceivers integrate the information they have about the number of times they have heard a sentiment expressed and the number of people they have heard express the sentiment.
Citation:
Kim Weaver, Stephen M. Garcia, Dale T. Miller, and Norbert Schwarz (2005) ,"Metacognitive Beliefs About Groups", in NA - Advances in Consumer Research Volume 32, eds. Geeta Menon and Akshay R. Rao, Duluth, MN : Association for Consumer Research, Pages: 307-308.
We look to others to tell us how to think, feel, and act. Researchers have a detailed understanding of how group norms affect group members cognition, emotion, and behavior. Surprisingly, however, much less is known about how people actually construct their estimates of collective sentiment (for notable exceptions, see Hamil, Nisbett, & Wilson, 1989; McFarland & Miller, 1990; Miller & McFarland, 1987; Prentice & Miller, 1993). At the same time, however, we know that perceivers often misjudge public opinion (Prentice & Miller, 1993). In this paper, we examine one reason why by exploring the process by which perceivers integrate the information they have about the number of times they have heard a sentiment expressed and the number of people they have heard express the sentiment. We propose that people use two important pieces of information when making group opinion judgments. First, perceivers have a feeling of familiarity for how many times they have heard an opinion expressed in a group, namely the opinions frequency. Because the more an opinion has been expressed, the more familiar it seems, repeated statements increase perceivers sense of familiarity for the opinion even when the expressions come from one person. Second, perceivers have conscious information about the range of group members making the opinion statements, that is, information about the extensity of the opinion. To form accurate group opinion inferences, perceivers must appropriately integrate familiarity information with information about the number of group members making the statements. We investigate whether people faced with repetition from one group member are able to appropriately integrate this information or instead, whether perceivers use their feelings of familiarity for an opinion to make inferences about the opinions extensity. Three studies examined peoples metacognitive theories about the relationship between feelings of familiarity for an opinion in a group and estimates of extensity, or the range of group members supporting the opinion. In Study 1, participants estimated a focus groups stance after either reading three comments from three different individuals (3 persons / 3 statements condition), three comments from one person (1 person / 3 statements condition), or one comment from one individual (one statement control). As expected, an a priori contrast showed that participants in the 3 persons / 3 statements condition estimated more focus group support for the issue at hand (M=6.2) than participants in the one opinion control condition (M=4.7), F(1, 174)=62.0, p<.001. More important, however, an a priori contrast also showed that participants who read one persons repeated statements in favor of the issue at hand (1 person / 3 statements condition) also estimated more focus group support for the issue (M=5.4) than participants in the one opinion control condition (M=4.7), F(1,174)=19.7, p<.001. Thus, the implication is that one individual repeating the same information can have almost as much influence as three different individuals each offering the same information. Study 2 examined boundary conditions of this repetition effect. We manipulated whether the norms around an issue were well-known ("evaluable") versus unknown ("unevaluable"). We predicted that the repetition effect would transpire when opinion estimators did not have previous knowledge about the issue at hand (unevaluable norms). However, when the issue was evaluable, opinion estimators would be able to discount the phenomenological feeling of familiarity brought about by repeated statements and consequently would not infer greater group support for the issue after reading repeated statements from one person. The results confirmed our prediction. Study 3 examined these differential effects of evaluable versus unevaluable norms in the presence or absence of a time delay. We predicted that after a time delay the repetition effect would transpire, even when participants had prior knowledge of the groups stance. In a between-subjects design, we found that the repetition effect transpires among opinion estimators when the issue at hand is unevaluable in both the presence and absence of a time delay. More important, however, we found that when participants have previous knowledge of a groups opinion (evaluable) they are initially able to discount the feeling of opinion familiarity brought about by one persons repeated statements (replicating the effect in Study 2). However, after a time delay, even participants who had previous knowledge of the group norm are affected by the repeated statements of one individual. Study 3 also investigated the relationship between observers estimates of group opinion and personal attitude change, and showed that when there was a time delay between exposure to one persons repeated counter-normative statements and the opinion measure, students shifted their own personal opinions in the direction of the "norm" they had misperceived. Follow up mediational analyses indicated that this personal opinion shift was due to misguided social influence. That is, students own opinions appeared to be driven by their perceptions of overall student support rather than changed directly as a result of repetition. These results demonstrate that using phenomenological feelings of opinion familiarity as the basis for judgments about the extensity of an opinion in a group can have important consequences, and under some circumstances can even lead people to change their own opinions in what may be seen as a misguided direction. REFERENCES Begg, Ian M., Ann Anas, and Suzanne Farinacci (1992), "Dissociation of processes in belief: Source recollection, statement familiarity, and the illusion of truth," Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 446-458. Hamil, Ruth, Timothy D. Wilson, & Richard E. Nisbett (1980), "Insensitivity to sample bias: Generalizing from atypical cases," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 578-589. Harkins, Stephen G. & Petty, Richard E. (1981), "Effects of source magnification of cognitive effort on attitudes: An information-processing view," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 401-413 Hsee, Christopher K., Loewenstein, George F., Blount, Sally, & Bazerman, Max H. (1999), Preference reversals between joint and separate evaluations of options: A review and theoretical analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 576-590. Jacoby, Larry L., Kelley, Colleen, Brown, Judith, & Jasechko, Jennifer (1989), "Becoming famous overnight: Limits on the ability to avoid unconscious influences of the past," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 326-338. Martin, Leonard L. (1986), "Set/reset. Use and disuse of concepts in impression Formation," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 493-504. McFarland, Catherine, & Dale T. Miller (1990), "Judgments of self-other similarity: Just like other people only more so," Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 475-484. Miller, Dale T., & Catherine McFarland (1987), "Pluralistic ignorance: When similarity is interpreted as dissimilarity," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 298-3.05. Prentice, Deborah A., & Dale T. Miller (1993), "Pluralistic ignorance and alcohol use on campus: Some consequences of misperceiving the social norm," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 243-256. Reber, Rolf, & Schwarz, Norbert (1999), "Effects of perceptual fluency on judgments of truth," Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 8, 338-342. Rothbart, Martin et al. (1978). "From individual to group impressions: Availability heuristics in stereotyping formation," Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14, 237-255. Schwarz, Norbert, & Clore, Gerald L. (1983), "Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513-523. Skurnik, Ian, Schwarz, Norbert, & Winkielman, Pytor (2000), "Drawing inferences from feelings: The role of naive beliefs," In H. Bless & J.P. Forgas (Eds.), The message within: The role of subjective experience in social cognition and behavior. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press. Strack, Fritz, Schwarz, Norbert, Bless, Howard, et. al. (1993). "Awareness of the influence as a determinant of assimilation versus contrast," European Journal of Social Psychology, 23, 53-62. Zajonc, Robert .B. (1968), "Attitudinal effects of mere exposure," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Monograph, 9, 1-27. ----------------------------------------
Authors
Kim Weaver, University of Michigan
Stephen M. Garcia, University of Michigan
Dale T. Miller, Stanford University
Norbert Schwarz, University of Michigan
Volume
NA - Advances in Consumer Research Volume 32 | 2005
Share Proceeding
Featured papers
See MoreFeatured
Changes in Social Values in the United States – 1976-2017: Is a New Age of Tribalism Emerging?
Eda Gurel-Atay, Independent Researcher
Johnny Chen, University of Oregon, USA
Wang Suk Suh, University of Oregon, USA
Lynn R. Kahle, University of Oregon, USA
Featured
Understanding the Role of Gifts in Managing Marriage and Family Relations: The Case of the Male Phoenix in China
Jia Cong, Lancaster University, UK
Xin Zhao, Lancaster University, UK
Chihling Liu, Lancaster University, UK
Featured
Consuming Commodified Selves – Accelerated Identity Co-Construction Dynamics Through Fashion Performances on Instagram
Jonathan David Schöps, University of Innsbruck, Austria