Consuming Impulses
ABSTRACT - This paper offers a psychological model of consumer impulse buying episodes. It goes beyond the standard definition of impulse buying (i.e. unplanned purchases) by distinguishing five crucial elements: I) a sudden and spontaneous desire to act; 2) a state of psychological disequilibrium; 3) the onset of psychological conflict and struggle; 4) a reduction in cognitive evaluation; 5) lack of regard for the consequences of impulse buying. A study of 202 adults was conducted. A consumer impulsivity scale was developed and then related to other consumer behaviors. Drawing upon depth interviews, we identified the distinctive elements that characterize the onset, structure, and psychological content of prototypic consumer buying episodes: product emanations, spontaneous urges to consume, the inner dialogue (cost-benefit analyses, resistance strategies, rationalization, guilt), and impulse persistence and power.
Citation:
Dennis W. Rook and Stephen J. Hoch (1985) ,"Consuming Impulses", in NA - Advances in Consumer Research Volume 12, eds. Elizabeth C. Hirschman and Moris B. Holbrook, Provo, UT : Association for Consumer Research, Pages: 23-27.
[Order of authors was determined by a flip of a coin.] This paper offers a psychological model of consumer impulse buying episodes. It goes beyond the standard definition of impulse buying (i.e. unplanned purchases) by distinguishing five crucial elements: I) a sudden and spontaneous desire to act; 2) a state of psychological disequilibrium; 3) the onset of psychological conflict and struggle; 4) a reduction in cognitive evaluation; 5) lack of regard for the consequences of impulse buying. A study of 202 adults was conducted. A consumer impulsivity scale was developed and then related to other consumer behaviors. Drawing upon depth interviews, we identified the distinctive elements that characterize the onset, structure, and psychological content of prototypic consumer buying episodes: product emanations, spontaneous urges to consume, the inner dialogue (cost-benefit analyses, resistance strategies, rationalization, guilt), and impulse persistence and power. THE SIGNIFICANCE OF IMPULSE BUYING .Many consumer products typically are classified as "impulse' items. What is it about these products that evokes the impulse label? It is not clear that a clear prototype exists, but impulse products are commonly characterized as low-cost, frequently purchased goods that demand little cognitive effort from the consumer. The implication is that impulse items elicit immediate and "mindless" reactive behavior (Langer, Blank & Chanowitz 1978; Langer & Imber 1980). Trade journals frequently note the spectacular sales results achieved through product displays and promotions that are geared specifically to encourage impulse purchases. Two typical examples of successful impulse-oriented marketing report a 400% increase in potato chip, cheese puff, and pretzel sales (Supermarketing Nov. 1977); and a 250% jump in razor blade sales (Supermarketing Jan. 1978). A "product" orientation has dominated much of the discussion about impulsive consumption. The trade journals emphasize the physical arrangements and modifications that seem to boost buying levels: brighter colors, point-of-purchase displays, placing products with "companion' items, special premiums. This perspective often leads to classifying of products into chose that are impulse items, and chose that are not; however, almost anything can be purchased impulsively. Expensive clothing, electronic equipment, books and lingerie might be considered non-impulse items, yet their purchase can Involve impulsive behavior. Consumer impulse buying is widespread, both across the population and across product categories. Bellenger, Robertson and Hirschman (1977) found that almost 40% of consumers' department store purchases fell into the impulse category, ranging from 27% to 62% of all purchases for each line. Few product lines were unaffected by impulse buying. Placing sole emphasis upon product type provides a limited perspective, since it is the individuals, not the products, who experience the impulse to consume. What sparks an impulse in one person may not in another. For example, each day millions of people pass by the National Enquirer as they file through supermarket checkout lines, but only a small percentage experience the urge to delve beyond chose energetically prurient headlines. Research needs to focus on the behavioral nature of these impulses to consume. In this paper, we first examine existing conceptualizations of impulse buying, and then offer our own definition. Next we present the results of an exploratory study that examined impulse buying behavior in 202 aduLts using both qualitative and quantitative methodologies. WHAT IS IMPULSE BUYING Past efforts at defining impulse buying have suffered because they have not incorporated the psychology underlying consumers' impulsive episodes. A theme dominating most of the work in marketing depicts impulse buying essentially as "unplanned-' purchase behavior (Applebaum 1951; Bellenger et al. 1977; Kollat & Willet 1967; Stern 1962). This is an easily observable and operational definition but it is quite limited (Levy 1970). From this perspective, an impulse purchase could be construed as any purchase not written on someone's shopping list. This definition could be retouched in information processing terms as the difference between "top-down' (on the list) and bottom-up' (not on the list) processing (Norman & Bobrow 1975). but this is where the idea of unplanned purchasing breaks down; in most cases cognition involves both conceptually-driven and data-driven processing. It is not accurate or useful to consider all the 'unplanned' purchases of a half-gallon of milk, a ten-pound bag of potatoes, or toilet paper as impulsive behavior. Clearly consumers use score layout as an external memory aid so the fact that a purchase is unplanned is neither a sufficient nor necessary (as we shall show) condition for construal as an impulse purchase. We have identified five crucial elements that distinguish impulsive from nonimpulsive consumer behavior. First impulsive behavior involves a sudden and spontaneous desire to act, representing a clear departure from the previous ongoing behavior scream. This notion of a rapid change in psychological states fits in well with neurophysiological representations, where an impulse is described as 'a wave of active change continuing along a nerve fiber' (Wolman 1977). In the same way that these neurological impulses trigger some biological response, psychological impulses can be viewed as stimulation agents driven by conscious and unconscious mental processes. For example, imagine that you are walking down the pickles and relishes aisle in the supermarket and notice a jar of marinated artichoke hearts; you feel a sudden urgency to buy them and go right home and build a huge antipasto. It is important to understand the difference between this exampLe and buying milk. Both are unplanned purchases brought about by visual stimulation, yet seeing the milk provides a convenient cognitive reminders while the artichoke hearts trigger a more complex response. The sudden urge to buy on impulse can throw the consumer into a state of psychological disequilibrium. This second feature of impulse buying can cause an individual to feel temporarily out-of-control. There is an extensive literature on the developmental and clinical aspects of impulsivity and impulse control. The ability to voluntarily refuse immediate gratification, to tolerate self-imposed delays of reward, is at the core of most philosophical concept, of "will power". The most fundamental steps in socialization require learning to control one's impulses and express them only under appropriate conditions, e.g. toilet training. The temptation to succumb to one's consumption impulses may threaten a person's budget, diet, schedule, or reputation. In these situations the consumer's disequilibrium may be substantial. In other instances impulsive consumption may represent spontaneous and creative activity, and involve much less psychological imbalance. The third element of consumer impulsivity is the psychological conflict and struggle that may ensue (Thaler & Shefrin 1981). Often the consumer feels ambivalent toward the products that are impulse objects. Freud (1920/1956) saw impulses as involving a struggle between two competing forces; the "pleasure and reality principles-' (the id and the superego). The consumer is pulled in two directions, and must weigh the benefits of immediate gratification against whatever long term consequences might result. Not all buying impulses necessarily involve conflict. The artichoke example could conceivably involve no conflict at all, but in many situations the "good" that arises from satisfying the impulse must be balanced with some later felt "bad". (Marinated artichoke hearts are fattening, and high in cholesterol and sodium.) Many conflicts occur because current consumption impairs one's ability to consume in the future. Because people tend to overvalue proximate satisfactions relative to more distant ones (Strotz 1956), the closer one is to being able to enjoy that immediate impulse, the harder it is to resist (Ainslee 1975). To illustrate, consider the following two impulses. 1) You are rummaging around the kitchen deciding what groceries you need to buy and you get a craving for Famous Amos cookies. 2) You are walking through a mall and you are instantly overcome by the aroma of freshly-baked Famous Amos Chocolate Chip Cookies. Suppose that you were trying to stick to a diet; which impulse would be the hardest to resist? Most likely it would be the latter impulse, because the urge can be satisfied so quickly. People often feel that impulses need to be indulged either right now or never. A fourth distinguishing aspect of impulse buying is that consumers will typically reduce their cognitive evaluation of product attributes. Weinberg and Gottwald (1982) believe that impulse buying involves distinctive transrational, affective states. Behavior is largely "automatic", high in affective activation, and low in intellectual control of the buying decision. Impulsive consumption is the antithesis of classical models of "economic man" as a rational expected utility maximizer, yet impulse buying is not mindless, low involvement behavior. In fact, we see impulsive buying as a most involving purchase behavior, at least for the moments right after the impulse arises. m e sudden urgency of the impulse requires the consumer's complete attention. Moreover, as conflict arises, cognitive activity may increase dramatically, depending upon whether the consumer has the motivation or ammunition to fight off the impulse. Finally, people often consume impulsively without regard to the consequences. Our framework must acknowledge the pathological aspects of impulsive consumption. Psychodynamic interpretations depict impulsivity as a form of neurotic behavior. Building on Freud's (1920/1956) model of civilization as based on impulse repression and sublimation, Reich describes impulsiveness as a "defect in repression (1925/1974). Grabbing the check-out line candy bar, the pretty blouse on sale, or the friendly lady at the cocktail lounge, may represent perfectly "normal" behavior, yet they could lead to bulemia, bankruptcy and herpes (or worse) respectively. Impulsiveness may deteriorate into a destructive character disorder (Kipnis 1977). Individuals with impulsive pathologies "seem to be living in a state of constant but stable chaos (with) little perspective about the future consequences of their current behavior' (Wishnie 1977). It is puzzling why people engage in dysfunctional impulsiveness, i.e. opting for smaller short-term rewards instead of holding out for larger long-term rewards. Ainslee (1975) offers three possibilities: a) we succumb to an impulse because we do not understand the consequences of our behavior; b) we know the consequences are bad but we feel impelled by some "lower" principle ("the devil made me do it"); c) we know the consequences but place too much weight upon satisfying present desires. Most people develop an elaborate repertoire of devices to control their impulses, ranging from placing the alarm clock across the room to opening non-interest bearing Christmas Club bank accounts. As we shall see, consumers also employ a variety of devices to control impulsive buying behavior. Even when the satisfaction of an impulse does not involve easily seen long-term negative consequences, people will often want to fight the temptation as a personal signal that they will be able to control themselves when it counts (Mischel 1971). A STUDY OF IMPULSE BUYING There were two purposes to this study. First, we wanted to explore the psychological content of consumer's self-reports of their impulse buying episodes. We developed an open-ended, "depth" interview instrument designed to extensively probe individuals' experiences with impulsive consumption. Second, we wanted to develop a scale of consumer impulsivity. Further, we wanted to investigate the relation between impulsivity, general attitudes toward shopping, attitudes toward shopping for particular types of products, and demographic characteristics such as age, sex. and income. Two hundred two individuals, half male and half female, were interviewed in their homes. The interview began with the depth interview; subjects were encouraged to express their feelings freely to a series of open ended questions and probes. Interviewers wrote down all responses. After this phase, subjects filled out a series of scales. The questions ranged from Likert-type items about general aspects of shopping to more specific questions about impulsive shopping behavior. In addition, subjects were asked to rate how much they enjoyed shopping for various types of products, followed by a short series of demographics. Completion times ranged from 3/4 to 1.5 hours, the average around one hour. The interviews were conducted in the Chicago and Los Angeles metropolitan areas. Respondents were broadly representative of the lower middle to upper middle classes, and were selected in equal proportion from late adolescent (18-24), young adult (25-35), and mature adult (over 35) populations. THE SCALING OF CONSUMER IMPULSIVITY Subjects rated 24 statements about shopping behavior according to how much each description applied to them. The responses were analyzed by principal components using an oblique rotation. Two clear factors emerged (48% of the variance). The first factor captured general attitudes toward shopping as an activity. (Representative items were "enjoy browsing', "enjoy shopping with friends", 'go shopping when depressed" and "don't consider shopping a chore .) The second factor focused on the impulsivity of a subject's shopping behavior ("Buy things spontaneously", "think credit cards are fun", "sudden urges to go out and buy something", and "often buy more than intended"). The 9-item SHOPPING scale and 8-item IMPULSE scale had reliability coefficients (alpha) of .899 and .864 respectively. (Details on the scales can be obtained from the authors.) The correlation between the two scales was .51 (p<. 001 ); impulsive shoppers tended to enjoy shopping more than those who were more cautious and Protestant in their buying styles. Individuals who scored high in impulsivity were more likely to: 1) like shopping at night (r=.33, p<.001), 2) enjoy shopping while "high" (r=.28, p<.001), and 3) like shopping by phone (r=.21, p<.01). These impulsive consumers also were less likely to schedule shopping on specific days (r=-.38, p<.001) or write out shopping lists (r=-.41, p<.001). These results characterize the impulsive consumer as a recreational shopper (Bellenger & Korgaonkar 1980) who shops when the mood strikes, finds gratification in shopping activities, and often buys more than planned. We also examined the relationship between these two scales and personal characteristics of the respondents. Consistent with earlier findings, females enjoyed shopping more than males, (c-4.6, p<. W l); they also were more impulsive, (t-2.29, p<.025). Many males felt that shopping was "a waste of time" unless it was "functional", i.e. they actually purchased what they had set out to buy. Men enjoyed buying what they considered "non-impulse, utilitarian" items like stereos, automobiles, appliances, and athletic equipment, (t=10.5, p<.001). Women enjoyed shopping for aesthetic goods like casual and dress clothing and grooming products (t=9.0mo <.001). The sex difference in consumer impulsivity could partly reflect the fact than men and women typically are shopping for different kinds of products. One additional finding was that younger people tended to be more impulsive in their shopping behavior than older people, (r=. 16, p-.02). Although this result is congruent with developmental theories of impulsivity (Mischel 1971), it needs to be investigated more systematically because changes in lifestyle and income (e.g. "empty-nesters", newly divorced) may influence this relationship. EXPERIENCING IMPULSE BUYING EPISODES From the depth interviews, we sought to develop a general model which could account for both the common process and content elements in consumers' impulsive episodes. Moreover, we wanted to relate this to previous research on impulsiveness and the five distinguishing characteristics of impulse buying developed earlier. To accomplish this, the following discussion reports findings that characterize prototypic consumer impulse buying episodes. Product Emanations An impulse buying episode begins with a consumer's sensation of some stimulus object, followed by a sudden urge to acquire it. Once an impulse is aroused, all attentional resources are focused upon the product. -- I have a real hard time in stores without things grabbing my attention. (F-28) [(Respondent gender is identified with an M or F, followed by age.)] -- I was in Field's and this blouse and skirt caught my eye. (F-22) Respondents often depicted themselves as innocents, minding their own business; they had not intended to buy anything when they suddenly fell victim to "subliminal" product emanations. -- I was in Beverly Hills just walking around not intending to buy when I saw some shoes on sale. So I went inside and tried them on and they fit fine. At that time I thought about buying one pair, then I got the feeling I had to cry everything. They were just calling to me. (F-24) -- I was standing in the grocery store checkout line, and the candy bar was staring there at me. (M-26) -- The pants were shrieking 'buy me', so I knew right then that I better walk away and get something else done. (F-35) These anecdotes suggest that these consuming impulses originate within the products. Consumers hint about magical, fantastic forces that animate some products. Independent of the objective reality involved, consumers talk of products that somehow mesmerize them to purchase impulsively, almost a case of willing seduction. In a sense these beliefs provide a basis for consumers to abrogate responsibility for actions that society might construe as juvenile or lacking in self-control. Products are imbued with wills of their own; the consumer may recognize the bad' involved in succumbing to an impulse to buy, but attributes this behavior to external forces (an example of "the devil made me do it syndrome). Consumers talk of their powerlessness to avoid the temptation, as if they were possessed by products, where only an immediate purchase could complete the marketplace exorcism. This is only one of many different "psycho-logics" (Abelson & Rosenberg 1958) that people use to maintain some semblance of rationality, at least to themselves (Levy 1981). Spontaneous Urges to Consume Impulse buying usually begins without conscious planning, arising spontaneously and without warning. Respondents describe being "suddenly overcome with a desire" to buy, coupled with intense urges to consume. -- I passed by a case containing brownies. I am depressed to begin with, so I bought four and ate them right there. I was glad I did it - it made me feel better. (F-33) This sudden desire to buy or consume is not casual; it moves quickly to center stage and demands immediate attention. -- You suddenly feel compelled to buy something. It feels like getting an IDEA. (M-22) -- I saw this wild Marimeko comforter and set of sheets. I saw how it could change my whole bedroom into a "hot jungle". (M-29) People may view their impulses as creative insights, or even spontaneous flashes of brilliance. Impulses are action-oriented; they trigger responses, usually quite quickly. Some individuals report that impulse buying episodes were of short duration. -- It's a fast feeling, and if I don't get it right away, I'll think of reasons why I don't need it. (F-32) -- It just happens very fast. If I like something that much, I will just buy it -- I don't need to chink about it much. (F-21) Many people expressed the feeling that their impulse purchases were unplanned. As mentioned earlier however, "unplannedness" is not a necessary condition for impulsiveness. In fact some people "plan on being impulsive. -- I grabbed $300 and went to Water Tower (Mall). I didn't really know what I was going to spend it on --- I didn't want to plan it out, because the best part of shopping for me is seeing something and knowing right away that's what I want. (F-28) By planning to be impulsive, people can enjoy the feeling of being overwhelmed by spontaneous urges and at the same time maintain some form of impulse control imposed by a budget. The Inner Dialogue Sometimes the impulse to buy stimulates the consumer to consider the probity of a prospective purchase. But even in apparently "mindless" situations, the consumer often engages in a serious inner dialogue. This section summarizes the cost-benefit analyses and various resistance strategies consumers employ in coping with their buying impulses. It also identifies the rationalizations and affective states that come into play. Cost-Benefit Analyses. People have developed broad repertoires for maintaining self control; they do whatever is necessary to regulate their shopping impulses. The most commonly mentioned means of impulse control involves reasoning with oneself, i.e. increasing the saliency of the negative consequences of the purchase (e.g., monetary constraints) or thinking about better ways to spend the money after saving up. -- Can I afford it, will I regret it? Will I get my utility, or use it once and forget? Do I really need it or want it? Is there a better way to use the money? (M-28) -- I think of other pleasurable things I could do if I could just hold off. (F-29) -- I have to be able to see myself using the product . Otherwise I'll resist. (F-28) Distancing Strategies. Although many respondents talked about the use of abbreviated cost-benefit analyses, it is not clear how effective people were in fending off their consumption urges. Jones and Gerard (1967) hypothesized that "time-binding" (the capacity to bridge the delay of gratification) hinges on self-instructional processes to increase the salience or dominance of the delay object, a form of self-reinforcement induced by actively anticipating future positive consequences. However Mischel and Ebbesen (1970) found that, at least with children, a better strategy was cognitive avoidance children could wait longer by engaging in self-distraction. Sometimes it is best to simply not think about it. -- You've got to walk away as soon as I feel an impulse, I immediately leave the area. (M-31) -- I try to distract myself by moving to another display. (F-41) -- I have a rule about eating - once I finish what is on my plate, I make myself wait half an hour before thinking about dessert. By that time, I'm usually doing something else or I'm not hungry anymore. I do the same thing when shopping. (M-34) -- I steer clear of record stores when I can't afford it. (M-24) -- Often five minutes cools me down. (F-35) People go to elaborate lengths to avoid tempting situations, essentially by playing little tricks on themselves (Schelling 1978). Small Rewards. Another means of impulse control involves placating oneself with a small purchase as a reward for resisting a bigger more costly impulse. --I had money to buy myself a gold chain, but I decided to buy it later. I bought a half-dozen roses for my girlfriend instead. (M-25) -- I almost bought this Gucci handbag, but I resisted. Later I bought some expensive chocolate, treating myself to luxury. It was "affordable" self-indulgence. (F-27) Precommitment. Respondents recognized that one of the problems with impulse buying is that what you want now may not be what you want later. Tastes and preferences change over time. -- I have a real hard time resisting clothes. My closet is filled with clothes I now regret buying. (M-31) -- To stop myself, I recalled my last impulse purchase that I never ended up using. (M-29) --I saw a pair of shoes I really liked, but I reminded myself I have many shoes I've never worn. (F-29) To avoid these problems, people employ various precommitment strategies (Strotz 1956; Thaler & Shefrin 1981), the classic example being Ulysses having himself bound to the mast so that he could listen to the Sirens while his crew sailed the boat with wax in their ears (Elster 1977). People impose rationing devices upon their behavior to preclude impulsivity (e.g., budgets or the use of chop sticks to avoid eating too fast). -- How do I discipline myself? I don't carry credit cards. (F-25) -- I bring little money with me. Going home for more cash gives me time to think. (F-26) -- When I go into stores I make a pact to go straight to the department that carries the thing I'm interested in. (F-32) -- I tell myself that I don't have the time to look at the product closely. (F-24) -- I make my boyfriend go with me. (F-31) Another precommitment technique involves the making of side-bets (Becker 1960), where future rewards are irreversibly tied to one's ability to avoid more immediate rewards. Lack of self control in the short run would lead to immediate forfeiture of a larger long-term reward. People are willing to place severe constraints on their immediate behavior (e.g. wiring one's jaws shut to lose weight) because they know they cannot trust themselves in the short run. Rationalizations. Another aspect of consumers' inner dialogues involves rationalizations of impulsive buying, the non-objective recoding of an impulsive purchase as non-impulsive. People are especially adept at maintaining cognitive consistency (vis a vis their own rationality). -- Clinique make-up - you get a "free" gift if you buy over $7 worth. I didn't need the product, but-I knew I'd use it. My friend bought it, so did I. ( F -24 ) -- Men's designer shirts at 40: off. I saw two I liked and charged them; it was hard to resist the good price, high quality, and besides I didn't need the cash. (M-33) -- I was out with some girlfriends; they were buying things and I felt deprived. So I bought nylons in various colors. I just felt compelled to buy something, but they were inexpensive and needed. (F-25) And sometimes the rationalizations do not even attempt to assume an air of rationality. -- My mind quickly starts rationalizing how much I need that product, and all the pros and cons go through my mind, except the pros usually outweigh the cons. (M-24) Guilt. Not all shopping impulses foment cognitive conflict, though many led to heated debates between the straight and wayward (impulsive) sides of a consumer. For some people the mere sensation of any impulse (shopping or not) is immediately met with a sense of guilt, an extreme form of the "Protestant Ethic', with its puritanical demands for self-restraint and its negative attitude toward pleasure (Mischel 1971). Spontaneous behavior is considered inherently bad and frivolous. -- I'm not big on impulses, I watch my money real close. (F-45) -- I'm from a conservative, rural background in which I was taught the value of a dollar. I don't spend money freely and always want the best value for my dollar. (M-31) -- Implicitly I know that impulse buying is not good. (M-39) -- I fear regretting the purchase later, and I think about what would happen if others found out how much it cost. (F-26) Impulse Persistence and Power Buying impulses add pressure to the inner dialogue because they are often persistent, and not easily dismissed by rational introspection. -- I didn't feel I could leave the store without the shoes. I imagined myself leaving and it just didn't feel good. (F-20) -- It gnaws at me until I buy it. If I want to get it I keep thinking about it. It won't get out of my mind until I buy it. (F-26) -- The feeling starts when I see something . . . it comes on very quickly and is a persistent nagging. (M-28) Individuals describe removing themselves from the area of temptation to another part of the store, but the buying impulse may continue to haunt chem. -- I saw this 14K gold ring on sale. Tried it on, took it off . . . tried it on, took it off. I left the store, but returned because all I could think about was how good it would look with painted nails, my white silk blouse, black pants and high heels. (F-25) At their strongest buying impulses are impossible to resist. Some respondents associate consumer-impulsiveness with pressing physiological drives or states. One person describes his impulse buying urges as feeling like a "hunger"; another describes the "tingling" that comes over her. Another young man reflects that his impulse buying urges "seem almost physical". The urgency to act is quite powerful and difficult to control. Respondents frequently use extreme terms to describe what it feels like when they experience the impulse to consume: exciting, risky, a "surge", fun!, naughty, great, happy, exhilarating, satisfying, compelling. CONCLUSION Ln this paper we have explored consumer's experiences with impulse buying. ';e have attempted to go beyond the view of impulse buying as nothing more than "unplanned purchases". The results were consistent with psychological interpretations, and reveal the complex psychodynamics that underlie impulse buying episodes. The current research is clearly exploratory but it has highlighted several important factors that require further study: I) how does mood influence impulse proclivity? 2) what situational factors stimulate or discourage impulse buying (e.g point-of-purchase)? 3) what resistance strategies are more or less effective and how can marketers use their knowledge of impulse control to "push consumers over the edge"? we also need to understand more about the trait of consumer impulsivity: how it is distributed across demographic and lifestyle segments; and how it is related to other forms of impulsive behavior. REFERENCES Abelson, R.P. & Rosenberg, M.J. (1958) 'Symbolic PsychoLogic: A Model of Attitudinal Cognition,' Behavioral Science, 4, 1-12. Ainslee, G. (1975) 'Specious Reward: A Behavioral Theory of Impulsiveness and Impulse Control,' Psychological Bulletin, 82, 463-496. Applebaum, W. (1951) 'Studying Consumer Behavior in Retail Scores,' Journal of Marketing, 16, 32-40. Becker, H.S. (1960) 'Notes on the Concept of Commitment,' American Journal of Sociology, 66, 32-40. Bellenger, V.N. & Korgaonkar, P.K. (1980) 'Profiling the Recreational Shopper,' Journal of Retailing, 56, 77-92. Bellenger, D.N., Robertson, D.H. & Hirschman, E.C. (1978) "Impulse Buying Varies by Product,' Journal of Advertising Research, 18, 15-18. Elster, J. (1977) "Ulysses and the Sirens: A Theory of Imperfect Rationality' Social Science Information, 16, 469-526. Freud, S. (1920/1956) "Beyond the Pleasure Principle," in J. Strachey & A. Freud (Eds.), The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 18), London: Hogarth. Jones, E. & Gerard, H.B. (1967). Foundations of Social Psychology, New York: Wiley. Kipnis, D. (1971) Character Structure and Impulsiveness, New York: Academic Press. Kollat, D.T. & Willet, R.P. (1967) 'Consumer Impulse Purchasing Behavior,' Journal of Marketing Research, 421-31. Langer, E., Blank, A. & Chanowitz, B. (1978) "The Mindlessness of Ostensibly Thoughtful Action: The Role of Placebic' Information in Interpersonal Inter-action," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 635-642. Langer, E. & Imber, L. (1980) "The Role of Mindlessness in the Perception of Deviance," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 360-367. Levy, S.J. (1970) 'The Discretionary Society', Proceedings of the American Marketing Assn. Levy, S.J. (1981) "Interpreting Consumer Mythology: A Structural Approach to Consumer Behavior," Journal of Marketing, 45, 49-61. Mischel, W. (1971) Introduction to Personality, New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston. Mischel, W. & Ebbesen, E. "Attention in the Delay of Gratification," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16, 329-337. Norman, D.A. & Bobrow, D.G. (1975) 'On Data-Limited and Resource-Limited Processes," Cognitive Psychology, 7, 44-64. Reich, W. (1925/1974) The Impulsive Character and Other writings, New York: The New American Library. Schelling, T.C. (1978) "Egonomics, or the Art of Self-management,' American Economic Review Papers and Proceedings, 68, 290-294. Stern, H. (1962) "The Significance of Impulse Buying Today," Journal of Marketing, 26, 59-62. Strotz, R.H. (1956) "Myopia and Inconsistency in Dynamic Utility Maximization,' Review of Economic Studies, 1956, 23, 166-180. Thaler, R.H. & Shefrin, H.M. (1981) "An Economic Theory f Self-Control," Journal of Political Economy, 89, 392-406. Weinberg, P. & Gottwald, W. (1982) 'Impulsive Consumer Buying as a Result of Emotions, Journal of Business Research, 10, 43-57. Wishnie, H. (1977) The Impulsive Personality, New York: Plenum Press. Wolman, B.B. (1977) Dictionary of Behavioral Science, New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold Co. ----------------------------------------
Authors
Dennis W. Rook, University of Southern California
Stephen J. Hoch, University of Chicago
Volume
NA - Advances in Consumer Research Volume 12 | 1985
Share Proceeding
Featured papers
See MoreFeatured
Time-of-Day Effects on Consumers’ Social Media Engagement
Ozum Zor, Rutgers University, USA
Kihyun Hannah Kim, Rutgers University, USA
Ashwani Monga, Rutgers University, USA
Featured
J6. Cozying up to the Kardashians: A Theory for Consumers' Affinity towards Celebrity Gossip
Jayant Nasa, Indian School of Business
Tanuka Ghoshal, Baruch College, USA
Raj Raghunathan, University of Texas at Austin, USA
Featured
Effortful but Valuable: How Perceptions of Effort Affect Charitable Gift Choice and Valuations of Charity
Haesung Annie Jung, University of Texas at Austin, USA
Marlone Henderson, University of Texas at Austin, USA